2015, Vol. 1(1): 43-52
University
students’ psychopathology: correlates and the examiner’s potential bias effect
Original Article
Helena Espírito-Santo, PhD (1a), Joana
Matreno, Psy M (1a)
(1) Instituto Superior
Miguel Torga, Coimbra, Portugal.
(a) Elaboration of the
work, collection and insertion of data, statistical analysis.
Corresponding author: Helena
Espírito-Santo; Largo de Celas, 1, 3000-132 Coimbra, Portugal; +351 910637946;
helenum@gmail.com.
http://dx.doi.org/10.7342/ismt.rpics.2015.1.1.10
Received 7 April 2014
Accepted 28 November 2014
ABSTRACT
Background:
Psychopathological
symptoms are very common among university students from different cultures, but
the socio-demographic correlates that have been found are different depending on
the studies. The correlates of such symptoms among Portuguese students have not
been studied thoroughly, besides the methodological issues that arise in this
type of studies, in the sense that has not been described who does the
psychological evaluations.
Aims:
The
main objective was to verify if there is a difference on psychopathological
symptoms between two groups questioned by two different examiners, controlling
for the potential role of social desirability, and other potential covariates.
Additionally, we want to assess the level of psychopathology and its
socio-demographic correlates.
Results:
The
level of distress was lower in comparison with other investigations. Women had
higher level of distress and more symptoms of somatization, anxiety, phobic
anxiety, obsessive-compulsion, and depression. The students that live a higher
distance from home had more anxiety and obsessive-compulsive symptoms. The
group assessed by the younger examiner scored higher in distress and in some
BSI factors, and had lower levels on social desirability.
Conclusions:
Sex
and distance from home seem important factors for university students’ mental
health. However, the examiner does have an influence in the evaluation,
probably because of social desirability, suggesting that the examiner’s
characteristics should be given in investigations involving university
students.
Keywords: Dissociation;
Examiner; Psychopathological symptoms; Students; Socio-demographic factors
INTRODUCTION
It is among young adults, particularly university
students, that most psychopathological symptoms are found (Mackenzie et al.,
2011; Mowbray et al., 2006), probably resulting from the transition from one
social role to another major social role (Parker, Summerfeldt, Hogan, &
Majeski, 2004; Taylor, Doane, & Eisenberg, 2013); to the potential changes
in lifestyle, friends, responsibilities, and geographical residence involved in
this transition (Taylor et al., 2013); to the greater number of university
students of diverse origins, some with economic disadvantages (Almeida, Soares,
& Ferreira, 2001; Kitzrow, 2003); and to the vulgarization of alcohol use
behaviors among this population (Corbin, Iwamoto, & Fromme, 2011; Greenbaum,
Del Boca, Darkes, Wang, & Goldman, 2005).
Many students starting university appear to have
personal and academic problems, which through the years has led to a
significant body of research documenting psychopathological symptoms in this
population (Allgöwer, Wardle, & Steptoe, 2001; Almeida et al., 2001;
Gonçalves, 2010; Greenbaum et al., 2005; Hoyt & Yeater, 2010; Jensen, 2003;
Kitzrow, 2003; J. D. Parker et al., 2004; Santos, Pereira, & Veiga, 2009;
Schwartz, 2006; Vannucci & Mazzoni, 2006; Zawadzki, Graham, & Gerin,
2013; Zoroglu et al., 2003). These problems seem to be particularly critical in
the first year of university (Cooke, Bewick, Barkham, Bradley, & Audin,
2006; Guo, Huang, Liu, & Wang, 2013; Read et al., 2012; Silveira, 2012),
and amongst medical students (e.g., Roberto & Almeida, 2011; Silveira,
2012).
Albeit some present a trend of decreasing rates of
students’ illness (review of Kraft, 2006), the high incidence of mental health
problems among university students is consistent across different cultures
(e.g., Guo et al., 2013; Hozoori & Barahmand, 2013; Mackenzie et al., 2011;
Nur, Cetinkaya, Sabanciogullari, Ozsahin, & Kavakci, 2008; Roberto &
Almeida, 2011; Steptoe, Tsuda, Tanaka, & Wardle, 2007; Verger, Guagliardo,
Gilbert, Rouillon, & Kovess-Masfety, 2009), pointing to developmental and
analogous social aspects of psychopathology.
Most of the studies reviewed above, when assessing
mental health, usually disregard the dis/similarity between researcher/examiner
and examinees. This is a question often present and took in consideration in
other contexts, such as the forensic (e.g., Melton, Petrila, Poythress, &
Slobogin, 2007), the counseling (Herring, 2002), or the testing context
(Urbina, 2004). In fact, age, gender, socioeconomic status, race or ethnicity,
temperament, expectations, proximity to the examinee and behavior during the
evaluation, are characteristics of the examiners that seem to affect the
results of testing (Ardila, 2005; Schafer, Papapolydorou, Rahman, & Parker,
2005). Additionally, self-report measures are commonly used in mental health
research, but they present a potential downside, such as the personal defense
acquiescence and the social desirability (deVries, 1992) making the type of the
examiner more relevant.
In fact, most of the research studies investigating
psychopathology with university students, out of university counseling centers,
did not indicate who the examiner was (or his personal characteristics) (e.g.,
Allgöwer et al., 2001; Greenbaum et al., 2005; Hoyt & Yeater, 2010; Hozoori
& Barahmand, 2013; Nur et al., 2008; J. D. Parker et al., 2004; Santos et
al., 2009; Schwartz, 2006; Taylor et al., 2013; Vannucci & Mazzoni, 2006;
DC WatsonBiren, 1999; Zawadzki et al., 2013), including when validating psychopathological
measures (L. S. Almeida et al., 2001; Cochran & Hale, 1985; Frazier &
Kaler, 2006; Gutiérrez Wang, Cosden, & Bernal, 2011; Koffel & Watson,
2010; David Watson & Wu, 2005), with rare exceptions (Guo et al., 2013;
Vickers et al., 2003).
This research
derives from the research of a senior investigator (Espirito-Santo, 2009),
followed by the study of a master student (Matreno, 2010). Therefore, we aimed
to verify if there was a difference in psychopathological symptoms in the two
samples collected by the two different researchers, one older and with a higher
academic status, and the other of the same age and academic status as the
students. In addition, our secondary goals were to assess the level of
psychopathology and its socio-demographic correlates among university students.
METHODS
Participants
Our
target population was the university students from two higher education
institutions at Coimbra. The total sample had 185 students, of which 66 (35.7%)
were male and 119 (64.3%) female. The average age of the students was 22.29
years (SD = 3.27; range: 18-40) and
the average study years was 13.65 (SD
= 2.02). These students were attending from 1st to the 5th
year of university in Law or Humanities (20.4%), Health and Psychology (54.5%),
Social sciences (13.2%), and Engineering areas (12.0%).
The total sample
was divided into two groups: one group was evaluated by a professor (Group A),
and the other group was evaluated by a researcher of the same age of the
examinees (Group B).
The group A was
one year older in average (Cohen’s d
= 0.53), ranging between 18-28 years (Median = 23; Group B: 10-40, Median =
21). There was also an association between the type of examiner and the civil
status, with group A having had significantly fewer participants with a partner
(boy/girlfriend or married). Finally, there was an association between the
education area and the type of examiner (Χ2 = 33.70; p < 0.001), with
Group A having had fewer Law, Humanities and Technology students, and Group B
fewer students from Social sciences. The groups had similar numbers of medical
students. There were no differences in the year of study between the two
groups. Students’ demographic characteristics are displayed in Table 1.
|
TABLE 1 Socio-demographics
of University Students Examined by a Professor (Group A) Versus
Examined by a Researcher of the Same Age (Group B) |
|
||||||
|
Variable |
Group A n = 87 |
|
Group B n = 98 |
t, Χ2 |
|
||
|
Mean (SD) |
% (n) |
|
Mean (SD) |
% (n) |
|
||
|
Age (years) |
22.80 (2.62) |
|
|
21.89 (3.71) |
|
2.03* |
|
|
Sex |
|
|
|
|
|
0.36 |
|
|
Female |
|
62.1 (54) |
|
|
66.3 (65) |
|
|
|
Male |
|
37.9 (33) |
|
|
33.7 (33) |
|
|
|
Civil
status |
|
|
|
|
|
24.87*** |
|
|
Without partner |
|
92.0 (80) |
|
|
60.2 (59) |
|
|
|
With partner |
|
8.0 (7) |
|
|
39.8 (39) |
|
|
|
Education |
13.84 (2.43) |
|
|
13.35 (1.49) |
|
1.62 |
|
|
1st
year |
|
42.5 (34) |
|
|
42.6 (40) |
0 |
|
|
≥ 2nd
year |
|
57.5 (46) |
|
|
57.4 (54) |
|
|
|
Residence
distance |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
≤ 1,5 hour |
|
74.7 (65) |
|
|
73.5 (72) |
1.50 |
|
|
]1.5 - 3] hours |
|
9.2 (8) |
|
|
14.3 (14) |
|
|
|
> 3 hours |
|
16.1 (14) |
|
|
12.2 (12) |
|
|
|
Note. t =
Student’s t test. Χ2
=
Chi-square test. *p <
0.05; ***p < 0.001. |
|
Procedures
Students were recruited from psychology
classes, social work classes, and from campus at Miguel Torga University College.
We told participants that the primary purpose of the study was to analyze
psychopathological symptoms, ensuring the confidentiality and the anonymity of
the data collected. Eligible students interested in participating in the study,
signed consent forms before completing the questionnaires (they did not
received any course extra credit for participating).
We collected the data between 2008 and 2010, using a
battery of three measures. For this study, we used only two instruments. The
respondents were assessed in small group sessions, involving 5 to 15 students
in small class/student rooms. We conducted the study in compliance with
appropriate internal review board.
Measures
A) Demographics. Participants filled a standard
demographic questionnaire asking respondents' age, sex, and year in school.
B) Brief Symptom Inventory (BSI). The BSI
(Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983) is a self-reporting inventory used to
measure current psychological symptoms and distress during the previous week.
It is a Symptom Checklist of 90 items (SCL-90) short version and has been
validated for the Portuguese population (Canavarro, 1999). It consists of 53
items on a 5-point Likert response scale, ranging from not at all (0) to
extremely (4). When answering, subjects are asked to appraise “the past 7 days
including today”. It evaluates nine dimensions of symptoms (somatization,
interpersonal sensitivity, anxiety, phobic anxiety, psychoticism,
obsessive-compulsive, depression, hostility, and paranoid
ideation) and three global indices (Global Severity Index, Positive Symptom
Total, and the Positive Symptom Distress Index). In the Portuguese version, the
author obtained a general population’s mean score of 0.84 (SD = 0.48), with the Cronbach’s alpha range in the sub-scales from
0.62 to 0.80 (Canavarro, 1999). Our study had a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.96 in the
total scale.
C) Marlowe-Crowne-2(10) Social Desirability
Scale (MC-2(10)/SDS). The MC-2(10)/SDS (Strahan & Gerbasi, 1972) is a
short form of Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale (Crowne & Marlowe,
1960) with 10 items. The MC-2(10)/SDS gives an estimate of socially desirable
responses as a potential source of evaluation errors. The total scores range
from 0 to 10, with higher scores related to more socially desirable responses
(Strahan & Gerbasi, 1972). In our study, we obtained a Cronbach’s alpha of
0.53 (acceptable in short forms (Barger, 2002).
Statistical
analysis
We
used the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS® 21.0 for Mac; SPSS Inc.
2012) to perform data analysis. We used the Kolmogorov-Smirnov test to check
the normality of distribution and thus make suitable statistical decisions.
We
converted raw BSI sub-scale scores to T-scores to understand if the levels of
distress were clinically relevant.
We
compared the two groups of students using the average scores of
psychopathological symptoms, and to do so we employed the Mann-Whitney U test.
For comparisons between the two groups using percentage results, we used as
appropriate with either the Pearson chi-square test or the Fisher’s exact test.
We
used Pearson, point-biserial correlations, and qui-square analysis to explore
the presence of associations between the BSI sub-scales and the continuous (age
and education years) and dichotomized socio-demographic variables (gender,
civil status, and education year: 1st year/≥2nd year).
The
effect of examiner on psychopathological symptoms was controlled for covariates
age, sex, civil status, education years, education area, and residence distance
through the Quade non-parametric ANCOVA (Marôco, 2014). This methodology was
used because there were differences in these covariates between the two groups,
and because they have a potential role on psychopathology (e.g., Shapiro &
Keyes, 2008; Springer, Pudrovska, & Hauser, 2011).
A
probability value of p < 0.05 was
considered to be statistically significant.
As currently
recommended (e.g., Wilkinson & The Task Force on Statistical Inference,
1999), we focused on effect sizes versus statistical significance and reported
both the amount of variance accounted for by group (η2) for the five domains, and standardized mean differences (Cohen’s d) for the separate variables. Partial
eta-squared for the ANOVA was calculated accordingly to the following formula: η2 = [F
(k - 1)] / [F (k - 1) + (N - k)],
where F is the ANOVA values, and k the number of groups (Richardson,
2011, p. 138). For interpreting partial eta-squared, 0.01 is a small effect,
0.06 is a medium effect, and 0.14 is a large effect (Ellis, 2010; Richardson,
2011). For Cohen’s d, 0.2 is small,
0.5 is medium, and 0.8 is large (Cohen, 1988; Ellis, 2010).
RESULTS
Descriptive
The subjects’ BSI
sub-scales scores and MC-2(10)/SDS score, and, respectively, the percentages
showing clinically significant levels of distress and high levels of desire of
social acceptance (T-scores > 60)
are summarized in Table 2.
|
Table 2 Brief
Symptom Inventory (BSI) Sub-scales and Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability
Scale MC-2(10)/SDS: Mean Raw Scores and Percentages of T-scores > 60 |
|
||||
|
|
Mean Raw Scores |
|
T-scores > 60 |
|
|
|
M |
SD |
|
% |
|
|
|
BSI |
|
|
|
|
|
|
Somatization |
0.39 |
0.48 |
|
10.4 |
|
|
Interpersonal sensitivity |
0.72 |
0.60 |
|
13.0 |
|
|
Anxiety |
0.66 |
0.54 |
|
12.8 |
|
|
Phobic anxiety |
0.33 |
0.44 |
|
14.1 |
|
|
Psychoticism |
0.51 |
0.50 |
|
13.5 |
|
|
Obsessive-compulsive |
0.97 |
0.61 |
|
15.7 |
|
|
Depression |
0.66 |
0.55 |
|
15.6 |
|
|
Hostility |
0.76 |
0.61 |
|
16.8 |
|
|
Paranoia |
0.80 |
0.61 |
|
14.4 |
|
|
Global Severity Index |
0.64 |
0.43 |
|
14.1 |
|
|
Positive Symptom Total |
23.69 |
12.18 |
|
17.6 |
|
|
Positive Symptom Distress Index |
1.35 |
0.32 |
|
15.3 |
|
|
MC-2(10)/SDS |
5.35 |
1.96 |
|
14.4 |
|
|
Note. M = mean; SD = Standard Deviation. |
|
After
removing the subjects that had T-scores above 60 on MC-2(10)/SDS, the BSI sub-scales
scores became slightly lower (the difference range from 0.06 to 0.2 for all
sub-scales, and 1.58 for Positive Symptom Total, with tpaired = 1.77, p =
0.104).
Correlates
Sex correlated with Global Severity Index (r = 0.16, p < 0.05), and symptoms of somatization (r = 0.15, p < 0.05), anxiety (r = 0.20, p < 0.01), phobic anxiety (r
= 0.20, p < 0.01),
obsessive-compulsion (r = 0.21, p < 0.01), and depression (r = 0.24, p < 0.01), with women having more symptoms (Global Severity
Index women: M ± DP = 0.70 ± 0.47;
Global Severity Index men: M ± DP =
0.53 ± 0.32; t = 2.63; p = 0.009; d = 0.42).
Civil status, and education years correlated only with
phobic anxiety (respectively, r =
0.20, p < 0.01; ϱ = -0.16, p < 0.01), with those that had no
partners (no partner: M ± DP = 0.28 ±
0.39; with partner: M ± DP = 0.45 ±
0.55; t = 2.24; p = 0.026; d = 0.64) and
those with less education having more phobic symptoms.
Residence distance correlated with anxiety and
obsessive-compulsive symptoms (respectively, r = 0.16, p < 0.05; r
= 0.15, p < 0.05): the longer the
distance, more the symptoms.
Course
year (1st year/≥ 2nd year), and years of age did not
correlate with any BSI subscales.
There was an association between education
area and some symptoms: interpersonal sensitivity (Χ2
= 15.59; p < 0.01; social sciences: 54.5% above
cut-off value), obsessive-compulsive (Χ2
= 13.12; p < 0.01; Law and humanities: 35.3% above
cutoff value), hostility (Χ2
= 10.31; p < 0.05; Law and
humanities: 38.2% above cutoff value).
Comparisons
In Table 3, we can see the average scores and the standard
deviations of the two instruments in the two groups defined by the examiner. We
can also verify that there were significant differences between both groups in
five sub-scales (Global Severity Index, phobic anxiety, psychoticism,
obsessive-compulsive, and Positive Symptom Distress Index).
Inspection of
Table 3 shows that Group B got an average score of 0.74 (SD = 0.50) in the
Global Severity Index accessed by the BSI, which was significantly higher than
that of Group A (M ± SD = 0.53 ±
0.28). However, regarding the BSI factors, there were only significant
differences in phobic anxiety (p =
0.001), psychoticism (p = 0.03) and
obsessions (p = 0.01). However, we
found significant differences in Positive Symptom Distress Index. We did not
find significant differences between groups (t = -1.46; p = 0.148) on
the MC-2(10)/SDS.
|
TABLE 3 BSI Subscales
and MC-2(10)/SDS: Differences of the Average Scores Between the Group
examined by a Teacher (Group A) and the Group Examined by a Researcher of the
Same Age of Examinees (Group B) |
|
||||||
|
Variable |
Group A (n = 98) |
Group B (n = 87 |
U |
d |
|
||
|
M |
SD |
M |
SD |
|
|||
|
BSI |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Somatization |
0.29 |
0.29 |
0.49 |
0.58 |
3657.0 |
0.44 |
|
|
Interpersonal sensitivity |
0.64 |
0.45 |
0.79 |
0.71 |
4010.0 |
0.25 |
|
|
Anxiety |
0.54 |
0.34 |
0.77 |
0.65 |
3618.0 |
0.44 |
|
|
Phobic anxiety |
0.20 |
0.27 |
0.44 |
0.53 |
3072.0** |
0.57 |
|
|
Psychoticism |
0.36 |
0.31 |
0.65 |
0.59 |
3198.5** |
0.62 |
|
|
Obsessive-compulsive |
0.78 |
0.43 |
1.14 |
0.69 |
3004.5** |
0.63 |
|
|
Depression |
0.56 |
0.40 |
0.74 |
0.65 |
3839.5 |
0.33 |
|
|
Hostility |
0.66 |
0.45 |
0.85 |
0.71 |
3809.5 |
0.32 |
|
|
Paranoia |
0.72 |
0.50 |
0.88 |
0.69 |
3834.0 |
0.27 |
|
|
Global Severity Index |
0.53 |
0.28 |
0.74 |
0.50 |
3405.0*** |
0.64 |
|
|
Positive Symptom Total |
21.98 |
10.92 |
25.21 |
13.06 |
3703.0 |
0.27 |
|
|
Positive Symptom Distress Index |
1.24 |
0.18 |
1.45 |
0.39 |
2929.5*** |
0.69 |
|
|
MC-2(10)/SDS |
5.49 |
1.95 |
4.83 |
1.95 |
901.0 |
0.34 |
|
|
Note. SD = Standard Deviation; U = Mann-Whitney’s U test;
d = Cohen’s d. BSI = Brief Symptom Inventory, MC-2(10)/SDS = Marlow-Crowe Social Desirability Scale. **
Significant differences at 0.01 level; ***Significant differences
at 0.001 level. |
|
After controlling the effects of covariate age, statistical
differences were maintained in Global Severity Index scores [FNon-parametric ANCOVA
(1,183) = 4.50; p < 0.05; η2
= 0.02], in phobic symptoms [FNon-parametric
ANCOVA (1,183) = 10.35; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.05], in psychotic symptoms [FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,183) = 9.17; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.05], in obsessive-compulsive symptoms [FNon-parametric ANCOVA
(1,183) = 12.04; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.06], and in Positive Symptom Distress Index [FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,183)
= 11.54; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.06].
After adjusting the effect of covariate sex, statistical
differences were maintained in Global Severity Index [FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,183) = 5.39; p < 0.05; η2
= 0.03], in phobic symptoms [FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,183) = 12.17; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.06], psychotic symptoms [FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,183) = 8.94; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.05], in obsessive-compulsive symptoms [FNon-parametric ANCOVA
(1,183) = 12.47; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.06], and in Positive Symptom Distress Index [FNon-parametric ANCOVA
(1,183) = 12.93; p < 0.001; η2
= 0.07).
After adjusting the effect of covariate civil status,
statistical differences were maintained in Global Severity Index [FNon-parametric ANCOVA
(1,183) = 7.08; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.04], in phobic symptoms [FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,183) = 8.65; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.05], psychotic symptoms [FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,183) = 12.57; p < 0.001; η2
= 0.06], in obsessive-compulsive symptoms [FNon-parametric ANCOVA
(1,183) = 13.33; p < 0.001; η2
= 0.07], but not in Positive Symptom Distress Index [FNon-parametric ANCOVA
(1,183) = 0.89; p = 0.348; η2
= 0.009).
After removing the effects of education area, phobic
symptoms [FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,165) = 6.08; p <
0.05; η2 = 0.04],
psychotic symptoms [FNon-parametric
ANCOVA (1,165) = 5.69; p < 0.05; η2
= 0.03], obsessive-compulsive symptoms [FNon-parametric ANCOVA
(1,165) = 8.65; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.05], and Positive Symptom Distress Index [FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,165)
= 8.51; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.05] remained statistical different between the two
groups, with exception of Global Severity Index [FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,165) = 2.61; p = 0.108; η2
= 0.02].
Education years did not affected the differences between
the two groups [Global Severity Index: FNon-parametric
ANCOVA (1,183) = 7.12; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.04; phobic symptoms: FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,183) = 14.23; p < 0.001; η2
= 0.07; psychotic symptoms: FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,183) = 10.82; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.06; obsessive-compulsive symptoms: FNon-parametric ANCOVA
(1,183) = 15.05; p < 0.001; η2
= 0.08; Positive Symptom Distress Index [FNon-parametric ANCOVA
(1,183) = 14.19; p < 0.001; η2
= 0.07).
The
same happen with residence distance [Global Severity Index: FNon-parametric ANCOVA
(1,183) = 5.80; p < 0.05; η2
= 0.03; phobic symptoms: FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,183) = 12.59; p < 0.001; η2
= 0.06; psychotic symptoms: FNon-parametric ANCOVA (1,183) = 9.47; p < 0.01; η2
= 0.05; obsessive-compulsive symptoms: FNon-parametric ANCOVA
(1,183) = 13.03; p < 0.001; η2
= 0.07; Positive Symptom Distress Index [FNon-parametric ANCOVA
(1,183) = 13.68; p < 0.001; η2
= 0.07).
After
the removal of the subjects that had T-scores above 60 on MC-2(10)/SDS, the
mean differences between the groups defined by the examiner were not
statistically significant anymore (p
> 0.05).
After
recoding scores into the two T-scores categories (under and above 60), we
encountered significant differences between both groups in all BSI sub-scales
(Table 4). It is also worth to note that high levels of social desirability
were more prevalent on Group A.
|
TABLE 4 BSI Subscales
and MC-2(10)/SDS: Differences in Percentages of T-scores > 60 Between the
Group examined by a Teacher (Group A) and the Group Examined by a Researcher of
the Same Age of Examinees (Group B) |
|
|||||
|
Variable |
Group A (n = 98) |
Group B (n = 87 |
X2 |
|
||
|
% |
n |
% |
n |
|
||
|
BSI |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
Somatization |
2.4 |
2 |
17.6 |
16 |
10.61** |
|
|
Interpersonal sensitivity |
1.3 |
4 |
20.4 |
20 |
10.20** |
|
|
Anxiety |
3.7 |
3 |
20.6 |
20 |
11.41** |
|
|
Phobic anxiety |
5.7 |
5 |
21.4 |
21 |
9.38** |
|
|
Psychoticism |
2.3 |
2 |
23.9 |
22 |
17.76*** |
|
|
Obsessive-compulsive |
5.7 |
5 |
24.5 |
24 |
12.25*** |
|
|
Depression |
6.2 |
5 |
23.7 |
22 |
9.89** |
|
|
Hostility |
10.3 |
9 |
22.4 |
22 |
4.84* |
|
|
Paranoia |
7.5 |
6 |
20.2 |
19 |
5.68* |
|
|
Global Severity Index |
2.3 |
2 |
24.5 |
24 |
18.79*** |
|
|
Positive Symptom Total |
10.5 |
9 |
24.0 |
23 |
5.70* |
|
|
Positive Symptom Distress Index |
3.4 |
3 |
26.0 |
25 |
17.98*** |
|
|
MC-2(10)/SDS |
18.4 |
16 |
0 |
0 |
5.16*a |
|
|
Note. SD =
Standard Deviation; U =
Mann-Whitney’s U test; d = Cohen’s d. * Significant differences at 0.05 level; **
Significant differences at 0.01 level; *** Significant differences
at 0.001 level. a Fisher’s exact test. |
|
After we removed the subjects that had T-scores above
60 on MC-2(10)/SDS, the significant differences remained for three BSI
subscales (Global Severity Index, psychoticism, and Positive Symptom Distress
Index;
respectively, Χ2 =
5.82, p < 0.05; 5.70, p < 0.05; 4.07, p < 0.05). The somatization, interpersonal sensitivity,
anxiety, phobic anxiety, obsessive-compulsive, depression, hostility, paranoid,
and TSP sub-scales were not statistically significant anymore (p > 0.05).
DISCUSSION
We proposed to
verify if there was a difference in psychopathological symptoms between two
groups of students when questioned by two different examiners, one of whom was
a teacher (Group A), and the other of whom was closer to them in age and
academic status (Group B).
Before that, we
proposed to analyze the level of psychopathology and its socio-demographic
correlates.
Generally, the
level of distress is lower comparing to other investigations that used a
comparable assessment methodology, but from different cultures (Cochran &
Hale, 1985; Pereda, Forns, & Peró, 2007; Watson & Sinha, 1999). We
cannot forget the importance of the culture differences, as Watson and Sinha
(1999) have showed, but this apparent contrast may be better understood if we
consider the role of the examiner.
Before that, lets
analyze the psychopathological correlates. Significantly, women have a higher
level of distress and more symptoms of somatization, anxiety, phobic anxiety,
obsessive-compulsion, and depression.
Sex differences in mental health were also evident in another studies
involving students of different cultures (Bayram & Bilge, 2008; Hardy et
al., 2012; Roberto & Almeida, 2011; Santos et al., 2009; Steptoe et al.,
2007; Taylor et al., 2013; Verger et al., 2009; Zinn-Souza et al., 2008), but
not in every culture (Guo et al., 2013; Nur et al., 2008). Could this be an
indication of less supportive relationships among our young women, even though
they are more likely to seek out and use social support (review of Taylor et
al., 2013)?
The students that
live at a higher distance from home have more anxiety and obsessive-compulsive
symptoms. Probably, these students have less supportive relationships, which
are especially important for mental health during demanding or transitional
times (Nur et al., 2008; review of Taylor et al., 2013). Also, loneliness is
more probable among these students, which is a source of considerable
psychological distress (Zawadzki et al., 2013). In fact, we found that those
students that are not engaged in an emotional relationship have more Phobic
anxiety. One study that analyzed displacement from home found that only the
combination of low income and living away from home was related with a higher
risk of major depression disorder (Verger et al., 2009). But, other Portuguese
investigation found the same trend of ours, showing that the displaced students
had more depressive symptoms (Santos et al., 2009), which could be explained by
the feeling of homesickness (Carden & Fiche, 1991; Ferraz & Pereira,
2002), and/or by the individualistic nature of our culture (Steptoe et al.,
2007).
Surprisingly
there was no association between course year (1st year/≥2nd
year) with any BSI subscales, contrasting with several investigations showing
that first-year college students report low levels of mental health (Cooke et
al., 2006; Guo et al., 2013; Read et al., 2012; Santos et al., 2009; Silveira,
2012; review of Taylor et al., 2013), although we have found that those with
less years of formal education have more phobic anxiety. The way Portuguese
students live their first year of university, very likely, reflects a cultural
aspect. Young people in Portugal work very hard for admission to the
university, and some may relax once this is achieved, dividing their academic
time with the very common engagement with social academic activities (like
“group initiation ceremonies”).
We also have
found that there was no association between years of age with any BSI
sub-scales, which is not consistent with some studies that revealed an association
between age and ego-resilience, depression, and self-esteem (Hardy et al.,
2012; Taylor et al., 2013), but is in line with results from other Portuguese
study, from a Turkish, and Brazilian research involving university students
(Nur et al., 2008; Santos et al., 2009; Zinn-Souza et al., 2008). These
disparate results might be due to different curricula, cultural differences, or
sampling errors.
A high prevalence
(54.5%) of students from social sciences have a high level of interpersonal
sensitivity symptoms, and a relevant number of law and humanities students have
a high level of obsessive-compulsive (35.3%) and hostility (38.2%) symptoms. Given the
different sample sizes, this should be investigated in further studies.
When we look at
the difference between researchers, we find higher scores in the sample
collected by the younger examiner. These differences are significant in the
Global Severity Index and in some BSI factors (phobic anxiety, psychoticism,
and obsessive-compulsive). There were no significant differences between
researchers in the values obtained in the MC-2(10) SDS. Notwithstanding these
considerations, if the results reflect a contextual response, then BSI, despite
of its instructions, is a potential measure of symptoms potentially induced by
situational assessment factors and potentially diminished by social
desirability.
The higher age of
the group of the older examiner does not seem to be responsible for those
differences. Besides, younger people are more prone to psychopathology (Hardy
et al., 2012; Steptoe et al., 2007; Taylor et al., 2013), the very opposite of
the older examiner findings.
Regarding the
psychopathological symptoms (Global Severity Index), our Group B has a mean
level of distress (M ± SD = 0.74 ± 0.50) similar to the mean found in Spanish
students (Pereda et al., 2007), American female students (Cochran & Hale,
1985), Asian male students (Iwamasa & Kooreman, 1995), and Israeli students
(Gilbar, 2002); but is higher than the mean found by Watson and Sinha (1999) in
Canada and India. Group A Global Severity Index mean score (M ± SD
= 0.53 ± 0.28) was lower than the means found in all the aforementioned
studies. Taking in account these comparisons, the assessment made by the
younger examiner seems more valid, but we do not know who the examiner in the
other investigations was. Again, we cannot forget the role of social
desirability in the differences between researchers in somatization,
interpersonal sensitivity, anxiety, phobic anxiety, obsessive-compulsive, depression,
hostility, and paranoid symptoms, except Global Severity Index, psychoticism,
and Positive Symptom Distress Index. Probably, social desirability was put into
action with the older examiner. But, if the results reflect a contextual
response, then BSI, despite of its instructions, is a measure of symptoms
potentially prompted by situational assessment factors and potentially
decreased by social desirability.
Another possible
explanation for the differences is that the younger examiner prompts the perceived
relationship between symptomatology and academic pressure.
Additionally, can
these results be explained by the authority of the examiner? In fact, according
to Ardila (2005) the examiner must be seen as an authority figure; but certain
personal characteristics such as gender, age, ethnicity, civil status, or
proximity to the individual, may undermine this authority. In many societies
social status is accorded to elders, so they are considered wiser and therefore
more reliable than younger persons, who are seen as less capable. That said, in
Western and other developed societies, the young are seen as having better
scientific and technological knowledge (Ardila, 2005); thus, the way the
examinee perceives the examiner varies from culture to culture.
These
hypothetical explanations disclose a hindrance of our study: if we have
included a diagnostic interview performed by a third party, we could tell more
accurately which one of the examiners obtained more reliable results? But then
again, who should play the role of this third party?
Contributing to
the uncertainty of these explanations, when we consider the prevalence,
research shows us again that the prevalence of psychopathological symptoms is
quite high in university students, and closer to the percentages found in Group
B (Allgöwer et al., 2001; Kitzrow, 2003; Santos et al., 2009). But, if we
compare with the only study conducted as a direct survey of a student sample
(revision of Reifler, 2006), the percentage is closer to Group A. Because of these
discrepancies, our results should be considered preliminary, demanding
replication in larger, and homogeneous samples.
Our study has
other limitations that should be noted. First, our sample was predominantly
feminine, calling into question whether the results are as applicable to
university males, in spite that sex differences are often minimized in
university subjects (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1987). Nevertheless, future research
should use homogeneous samples to control the potential moderator role of sex.
We did not
control the socioeconomic background, life stress, and social support, which is
another hindrance, since these variables have also been demonstrated to be
related to mental health during transition to the university (Steptoe et al.,
2007; review of Taylor et al., 2013; Verger at al., 2009). Further studies
analyzing the correlates of students’ mental health should take socioeconomic
background into account.
Finally, since
the present study was partially correlational and descriptive in nature,
findings do not imply causal explanations among variables.
CONCLUSIONS
Given the
elevated risk of dropping out of university among students with poor mental
health (Hartley, 2012), and the high rates of suicide in this population
(Mackenzie et al., 2011; review of Mowbray et al., 2006; Zoroglu et al., 2003),
this type of studies is important for targeting interventions. We have found
potentially important factors for the variation of university students’ mental
health, and the importance of the circumstances of the assessment, indicating
that at the very least the examiner’s personal or socio-demographic
characteristics should be given in this type of research.
An additional
conclusion concerns the instrument used for the assessment of mental health.
Investigations using BSI must be cautious on interpreting its scores as a
reflection of symptomatology of people experiencing psychiatric problems and
consider that disparate patterns in BSI scales could simply be methodological
effects. For validation of results, a measure of social desirability should be
part of assessment protocols with this population.
Conflito de interesses | Conflict of interest: nenhum | none.
Fontes de financiamento | Funding sources: nenhuma | none.
REFERENCES
Allgöwer,
A., Wardle, J., & Steptoe, A. (2001). Depressive symptoms, social support,
and personal health behaviors in young men and women. Health Psychology, 20(3), 223-227.
doi:10.1037/0278-6133.20.3.223 [Google Scholar]
Almeida,
L. S., Soares, A. P., & Ferreira, J. A. (2001). Adaptação, rendimento e
desenvolvimento dos estudantes no ensino superior: Construção do Questionário de
Vivências Académicas [Adaptation, performance, and development of college
students. Construction of the Academic
Experiences Questionnaire]. Methodus: Revista Científica e Cultural, 3(5), 3-20. Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/1822/12082 [Google Scholar]
Ardila,
A. A. (2005). Cultural values underlying psychometric cognitive testing. Neuropsychology Review, 15(4), 185-195. doi:10.1007/s11065-005-9180-y [Google Scholar]
Barger,
S. D. (2002). The Marlowe-Crowne affair: short forms, psychometric structure,
and social desirability. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 79(2), 286-305.
doi:10.1207/S15327752JPA7902_11 [Google Scholar]
Bayram,
N., & Bilgel, N. (2008). The prevalence of socio-demographic correlations
of depression, anxiety and stress among a group of university students. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 43, 667-672. doi:10.1007/s00127-008-0345-x [Google Scholar]
Canavarro,
M. C. (1999) Inventário de sintomas psicopatológicos (BSI). Uma revisão crítica
dos estudos realizados em Portugal [Brief Symptom Inventory/BSI. A critical
review of studies conducted in Portugal]. In M. R. Simões, M. M. Gonçalves,
& L. Almeida (Eds.), Avaliação
Psicológica. Instrumentos validados para a população portuguesa (Vol. 3, pp. 305-330). Coimbra: Quarteto. [Google Scholar]
Carden,
A. I., & Feicht, R. (1991). Homesickness among American and Turkish College
Students. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 22(3), 418-428. doi:10.1177/0022022191223007 [Google Scholar]
Cochran,
C. D., & Hale, W. D. (1985). College student norms on the Brief Symptom
Inventory. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41(6), 777-779. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(198511)41:6<777::aid-jclp2270410609>3.0.co;2-2 [Google Scholar]
Cohen,
J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
Cooke,
R., Bewick, B. M., Barkham, M., Bradley, M., & Audin, K. (2006). Measuring,
monitoring and managing the psychological well-being of first year university
students. British Journal of Guidance & Counselling, 34(4), 505-517. doi:10.1080/03069880600942624 [Google Scholar]
Corbin,
W. R., Iwamoto, D. K., & Fromme, K. (2011). Broad social motives, alcohol
use, and related problems: Mechanisms of risk from high school through college. Addictive Behaviors, 36(3), 222-230. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.11.004 [Google Scholar]
Crowne,
D. P., & Marlowe, D. (1960). A new scale of social desirability independent
of psychopathology. Journal of
Consulting Psychology, 24(4), 349-354.
doi:10.1037/h0047358 [Google Scholar]
Derogatis,
L. R., & Melisaratos, N. (1983). The Brief Symptom Inventory: an
introductory report. Psychological
Medicine, 13(03), 595-605.
doi:10.1017/s0033291700048017 [Google Scholar]
deVries,
M. W. (1992). The experience of psychopathology: Investigating mental
disorders in their natural settings.
Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
Ellis,
P. D. (2010). The essential guide to effect sizes. Statistical power,
meta-analysis, and the interpretation of research results. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
Espirito-Santo,
H. (2009). Histeria: A unidade perdida. Estudo dos fenómenos semelhantes e
dissemelhantes das perturbações somatoformes e dissociativas [Hysteria: The lost unity. Study of
similar and dissimilar phenomena of the dissociative and somatoform disorders]
(Doctoral dissertation, Instituto de Ciências Biomédicas Abel Salazar, Porto). Retrieved from http://hdl.handle.net/10216/19354 [Google Scholar]
Ferraz,
M. F., & Pereira, A. S. (2002). A dinâmica da personalidade e o
homesickness (saudades de casa) dos jovens estudantes universitários
[Personality and homesickness in young college students]. Psicologia, Saúde & Doenças, 3(2), 149-164. Retrieved from
http://www.scielo.mec.pt/scielo.php?script=sci_arttext&pid=S1645-00862002000200004 [Google Scholar]
Frazier,
P. A., & Kaler, M. E. (2006). Assessing the validity of self-reported
stress-related growth. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74(5), 859-869. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.74.5.859 [Google Scholar]
Gilbar,
O. (2002). Adult Israeli community norms for the Brief Symptom inventory (BSI). International Journal of Stress Management, 9(1), 1-10. doi:10.1023/a:1013097816238 [Google Scholar]
Gonçalves,
L. M. C. (2010). Saudades de casa,
depressão e personalidade em estudantes universitários [Homesickness, depression, and
personality in college students] (Master's thesis, Instituto Superior Miguel
Torga, Coimbra). Retrieved from
http://repositorio.ismt.pt/handle/123456789/268 [Google Scholar]
Greenbaum,
P. E., Del Boca, F. K., Darkes, J., Wang, C. P., & Goldman, M. S. (2005).
Variation in the drinking trajectories of freshmen college students. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(2), 229-238. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.73.2.229 [Google Scholar]
Guo, Q.,
Huang, Y., Liu, Z., & Wang, H. (2013). Psychological symptoms and
associated risk factors in Chinese freshmen: A three-year follow-up study. Chinese Medical Journal, 126(18), 3499-3504. doi:10.3760/cma.j.issn.0366-6999.20131201 [Google Scholar]
Gutiérrez
Wang, L., Cosden, M., & Bernal, G. (2011). Adaptation and validation of the
Spanish-language Trauma Symptom Inventory in Puerto Rico. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(1), 118-122. doi:10.1037/a0021327 [Google Scholar]
Hardy,
S. A., Francis, S. W., Zamboanga, B. L., Kim, S. Y., Anderson, S. G., &
Forthun, L. F. (2012). The roles of identity formation and moral identity in
college student mental health, health-risk behaviors, and psychological
well-being. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 69(4), 364-382. doi:10.1002/jclp.21913 [Google Scholar]
Hartley,
M. T. (2012). Assessing and promoting resilience: An additional tool to address
the increasing number of college students with psychological problems. Journal of College Counseling, 15, 37-51. doi:10.1002/j.2161-1882.2012.00004 [Google Scholar]
Herring,
R. D. (2002) Multicultural counseling for career development. In J. Trusty, E.
J. Looby, & D. S. Sandhu (Eds.), Multicultural
counseling: Context, theory and practice, and competence (pp. 219-246). New York: Nova Science Publishers, Inc. [Google Scholar]
Hoyt,
T., & Yeater, E. A. (2010). Comparison of posttraumatic stress disorder
symptom structure models in Hispanic and White college students. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy, 2(1), 19-30. doi:10.1037/a0018745 [Google Scholar]
Hozoori,
R., & Barahmand, U. (2013). A study of the relationship of alexithymia and dissociative
experiences with anxiety and depression in students. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 84, 128-133. doi:10.1016/j.sbspro.2013.06.522 [Google Scholar]
Iwamasa,
G. Y., & Kooreman, H. (1995). Brief Symptom Inventory scores of Asian,
Asian-American, and European-American college students. Cultural Diversity and Mental Health, 1(2), 149-157. doi:10.1037/1099-9809.1.2.149 [Google Scholar]
Jensen,
D. R. (2003). Understanding sleep disorders in a college student population. Journal of College Counseling, 6(1), 25-34. doi:10.1002/j.2161-1882.2003.tb00224.x [Google Scholar]
Kitzrow,
M. A. (2003). The mental health needs of today’s college students: Challenges
and recommendations. NASPA Journal, 41(1), 167-181. doi:10.2202/1949-6605.1310 [Google Scholar]
Koffel,
E., & Watson, D. (2010). Development and initial validation of the Iowa Sleep
Disturbances Inventory. Assessment, 17(4), 423-439. doi:10.1177/1073191110362864 [Google Scholar]
Kraft,
D. P. (2006). College health psychiatry 40 years later. Journal of American College Health, 54(6), 315-316. doi:10.3200/JACH.54.6.315-316 [Google Scholar]
Mackenzie,
S., Wiegel, J. R., Mundt, M., Brown, D., Saewyc, E., Heiligenstein, E., . . .
Fleming, M. (2011). Depression and suicide ideation among students accessing
campus health care. American Journal of
Orthopsychiatry, 81(1), 101-107.
doi:10.1111/j.1939-0025.2010.01077.x [Google Scholar]
Marôco,
J. (2014). Análise Estatística com o SPSS Statistics [Statistical Analysis with SPSS
Statistics] (5th ed.). Pero Pinheiro:
ReportNumber. [Google Scholar]
Matreno,
J. (2010). Experiências dissociativas e saudades de casa em estudantes do
Ensino Superior [Dissociative
experiences and homesickness in college students] (Master's thesis, Instituto
Superior Miguel Torga, Coimbra). Retrieved from
http://repositorio.ismt.pt/handle/123456789/217 [Google Scholar]
Melton,
G. B., Petrila, J., Poythress, N. G., & Slobogin, C. (2007). Psychological evaluations for the courts: A handbook for mental
health professionals and lawyers (3rd ed.). New
York: Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
Mowbray,
C. T., Megivern, D., Mandiberg, J. M., Strauss, S., Stein, C. H., Collins, K.,
. . . Lett, R. (2006). Campus mental health services: recommendations for
change. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 76(2), 226-237. doi:10.1037/0002-9432.76.2.226 [Google Scholar]
Nolen-Hoeksema,
S. (1987). Sex differences in unipolar depression: Evidence and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 101(2), 259-282. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.259 [Google Scholar]
Nur, N.,
Cetinkaya, S., Sabanciogullari, V., Ozsahin, S. L., & Kavakci, O. (2008).
Emotional disorders among Turkish undergraduate medical students. Pakistan Journal of Medical Science, 24, 792-797. Retrieved from https://www.pjms.com.pk/issues/octdec208/article/article4.html [Google Scholar]
Parker,
J. D., Summerfeldt, L. J., Hogan, M. J., & Majeski, S. A. (2004). Emotional
intelligence and academic success: Examining the transition from high school to
university. Personality and Individual Differences, 36(1), 163-172. doi:10.1016/s0191-8869(03)00076-x [Google Scholar]
Pereda,
N., Forns, M., & Peró, M. (2007). Dimensional structure of the Brief
Symptom Inventory with Spanish college students. Psicothema, 19(4), 634-639. Retrieved
from http://www.psicothema.com/PDF/3409.pdf [Google Scholar]
Read, J.
P., Colder, C. R., Merrill, J. E., Ouimette, P., White, J., & Swartout, A.
(2012). Trauma and posttraumatic stress symptoms predict alcohol and other drug
consequence trajectories in the first year of college. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 80(3), 426-439. doi:10.1037/a0028210 [Google Scholar]
Richardson,
J. T. E. (2011). Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size
in educational research. Educational Research
Review, 6(2), 135-147. doi:10.1016/j.edurev.2010.12.001 [Google Scholar]
Roberto,
A., & Almeida, A. (2011). A saúde mental de estudantes de medicina: estudo
exploratório na Universidade da Beira Interior [Mental health of students of
medicine: Exploratory study in the Universidade da. Beira Interior]. Acta Médica Portuguesa, 24, 279-286. Retrieved from
https://www.actamedicaportuguesa.com/revista/index.php/amp/article/view/1490/1076 [Google Scholar]
Schwartz,
A. J. (2006). Are college students more disturbed today? Stability in the
acuity and qualitative character of psychopathology of college counseling
center clients: 1992-1993 through 2001-2002. Journal of American College Health, 54(6), 327-337. doi:10.3200/JACH.54.6.327-337 [Google Scholar]
Shapiro,
A., & Keyes, C. L. M. (2008). Marital status and social well-being: Are the
married always better off?. Social Indicators
Research, 88(2), 329-346. doi:10.1007/s11205-007-9194-3 [Google Scholar]
Silveira,
C., Norton, A., Brandão, I., & Roma-Torres, A. (2012). Saúde mental em
estudantes do ensino superior. Experiência da consulta de psiquiatria do centro
hospitalar São João [Mental health of college students: Experience of the
University Psychiatric Outpatient Clinic of Hospital de São João. Acta Médica Portuguesa, 24(S2), 247-256. Retrieved from
https://www.actamedicaportuguesa.com/revista/index.php/amp/article/view/1504/1089 [Google Scholar]
Springer,
K. W., Pudrovska, T., & Hauser, R. M. (2011). Does psychological well-being
change with age? Longitudinal tests of age variations and further exploration
of the multidimensionality of Ryff's model of psychological well-being. Social Science Research, 40(1), 392-398. doi:10.1016/j.ssresearch.2010.05.008 [Google Scholar]
Steptoe,
A., Tsuda, A., Tanaka, Y., & Wardle, J. (2007). Depressive symptoms,
socio-economic background, sense of control, and cultural factors in university
students from 23 countries. International
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 14(2), 97-107.
doi:10.1007/bf03004175 [Google Scholar]
Strahan,
R., & Gerbasi, K. C. (1972). Short, homogeneous versions of the
Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 28(2), 191-193. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(197204)28:2<191::AID-JCLP2270280220>3.0.CO;2-G [Google Scholar]
Taylor,
Z. E., Doane, L. D., & Eisenberg, N. (2013). Transitioning from high school
to college: Relations of social support, ego-resiliency, and maladjustment
during emerging adulthood. Emerging Adulthood, 2(2), 105-115. doi:10.1177/2167696813506885 [Google Scholar]
Urbina,
S. (2004). Essentials of psychological testing. New Jersey: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
Vannucci,
M., & Mazzoni, G. (2006). Dissociative experiences and mental imagery in
undergraduate students: When mental images are used to foresee uncertain future
events. Personality and Individual Differences, 41(6), 1143-1153. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.02.021 [Google Scholar]
Verger,
P., Guagliardo, V., Gilbert, F., Rouillon, F., & Kovess-Masfety, V. (2009).
Psychiatric disorders in students in six French universities: 12-month
prevalence, comorbidity, impairment and help-seeking. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 45(2), 189-199. doi:10.1007/s00127-009-0055-z [Google Scholar]
Vickers,
K. S., Patten, C. A., Lane, K., Clark, M. M., Croghan, I. T., Schroeder, D. R.,
& Hurt, R. D. (2003). Depressed versus nondepressed young adult tobacco
users: Differences in coping style, weight concerns and exercise level. Health Psychology, 22(5), 498-503.
doi:10.1037/0278-6133.22.5.498 [Google Scholar]
Watson,
D., & Wu, K. D. (2005). Development and validation of the Schedule of
Compulsions, Obsessions, and Pathological Impulses (SCOPI). Assessment, 12(1), 50-65.
doi:10.1177/1073191104271483 [Google Scholar]
Watson,
D. C., & Sinha, B. C. (1999). A cross-cultural comparison of the Brief
Symptom Inventory. International
Journal of Stress Management, 6(4), 255-264.
doi:10.1023/a:1021940321129 [Google Scholar]
Zawadzki,
M. J., Graham, J. E., & Gerin, W. (2013). Rumination and anxiety mediate
the effect of loneliness on depressed mood and sleep quality in college
students. Health Psychology, 32(2), 212-222.
doi:10.1037/a0029007 [Google Scholar]
Zinn-Souza,
L. C., Nagai, R., Teixeira, L. R., Latorre, M., Roberts, R., Cooper, S. P.,
& Fischer, F. M. (2008). Factors associated with depression symptoms in
high school students in São Paulo, Brazil. Revista de Saúde Pública, 42(1), 34-40. doi:10.1590/S0034-89102008000100005 [Google Scholar]
Zoroglu,
S. S., Tuzun, U., Sar, V., Tutkun, H., Savaçs, H. A., Ozturk, M., . . . Kora,
M. E. (2003). Suicide attempt and self-mutilation among Turkish high school
students in relation with abuse, neglect and dissociation. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 57(1), 119-126. doi:10.1046/j.1440-1819.2003.01088.x [Google Scholar]