Revista Portuguesa de Investigação Comportamental e Social 2019 Vol. 6 (2): 56-68
Portuguese Journal of
Behavioral and Social Research 2019 Vol. 6 (2): 56-68
Departamento de Investigação & Desenvolvimento • Instituto
Superior Miguel Torga
ARTIGO ORIGINAL
Feelings
of shame and the psychological well-being of adolescents: Fears of receiving
compassion and social safeness as mediating processes
A vergonha e o bem-estar psicológico dos adolescentes: O medo de receber compaixão e os sentimentos de segurança e ligação aos outros como processos mediadores
Ana Laura Mendes (1)
Maria Cristina Canavarro (1)
Cláudia Ferreira (1)
(1) Faculty of
Psychological and Education Sciences, University of Coimbra, Center for
Research in Neuropsychology and Cognitive and Behavioral Intervention
(CINEICC), Portugal
Recebido: 30/09/2020; Revisto: 02/11/2020; Aceite: 11/11/2020.
https://doi.org/10.31211/rpics.2020.6.2.187
Objectives: The current study
aims to test the mediating role of fears of receiving compassion from others
and of current feelings of social safeness and connectedness in the association
between external shame and the psychological well-being of adolescents. We
hypothesize that adolescents with higher levels of external shame may have
lower psychological well-being indicators due to increased fear of receiving
compassion from others and decreased feelings of social safeness. Methods:
A total of 361 Portuguese adolescents of both sexes (43.8% boys; 56.2% girls),
aged between 12 and 18 participated in this study by completing self-report
measures to evaluate feelings of shame, fears of compassion, current feelings
of social safeness, and the psychological well-being of adolescents. Data were
analyzed through descriptive and correlational statistics, and the suitability
of the model was tested via path analysis. Results: Results seemed to
demonstrate that, in both sexes, adolescents’ external shame is associated with
psychological well-being. This effect was mediated by increased fear of
receiving others' compassion and decreased connection and security feelings in
social contexts. Specifically, results indicated that the tested model presented
an adequate adjustment to the data, explaining 15.0% of the variance of fears
of receiving compassion from others, 37.0% of the variance of current feelings
of social safeness, and 46.0% of the variance of the psychological well-being
of adolescents. Conclusions: This study contributes to a better
understanding of the role that feelings
of inferiority, fears of receiving compassion from others, and difficulty
establishing safe and pleasurable relationships with others, in the social
context, have on the psychological well-being of adolescents of both sexes.
Keywords: Adolescents;
External shame; Fears of compassion; Psychological well-being; Social safeness;
Exploratory study.
Resumo
Objetivos:
O objetivo do presente estudo foi testar o
papel mediador do medo de receber compaixão dos outros e dos sentimentos de
segurança e proximidade, em contexto social, na associação entre vergonha
externa e o bem-estar psicológico dos adolescentes. Foi hipotetizado
que adolescentes com elevados níveis de vergonha externa apresentem uma
diminuição nos níveis de bem-estar psicológico, através do aumento do medo de
receber compaixão e da diminuição dos sentimentos de segurança e proximidade em
contexto social. Métodos: Participaram neste estudo 361 adolescentes
portugueses de ambos os sexos (43,8% rapazes; 56,2% raparigas), com idade entre
os 12 e os 18 anos, os quais completaram medidas de autorresposta relativamente
a sentimentos de vergonha, medo da compaixão, sentimentos de segurança e de
proximidade aos outros, e bem-estar percebido dos adolescentes. Os dados foram explorados através de
estatísticas descritivas e correlacionais, e o modelo teórico proposto foi testado
através de análises path. Resultados: Os
dados pareceram demonstrar que, em ambos os sexos, a vergonha externa está
associada a um decréscimo do bem-estar psicológico dos adolescentes, e que este
efeito é mediado pelo aumento do medo de receber compaixão dos outros e pela
diminuição dos sentimentos de segurança e conexão em contexto social. Os
resultados indicaram que o modelo testado apresenta um adequado ajustamento aos
dados, explicando 15,0% da variância do medo de receber compaixão dos outros,
37,0% da variância dos sentimentos de segurança e proximidade aos outros e 46,0%
da variância do bem-estar psicológico dos adolescentes. Conclusões: Este
estudo parece contribuir para uma compreensão mais aprofundada acerca da
importância do papel dos sentimentos de inferioridade, do medo de receber
compaixão dos outros, e dos sentimentos de segurança e proximidade, no contexto
social, no bem-estar psicológico dos adolescentes.
Palavras-Chave: Adolescentes;
Vergonha externa; Medo da compaixão; Sentimentos de segurança e ligação aos
outros; Bem-estar psicológico; Estudo exploratório.
Literature has shown
an increasing interest in the study of quality of life in children and
adolescents. Quality of life is considered an indicator of general well-being
and is commonly defined by the World Health Organization (2020) as a
multidimensional construct related to physical, emotional, mental, and social
well-being. Quality of life in adolescence is particularly important since this
developmental phase is characterized by several and crucial changes (Proctor et al., 2009; Salmela-Aro & Tuominen-Soini, 2010) that might influence
their perception of health-related dimensions (Gaspar et al., 2012).
The series of
psychosocial transformations that characterize adolescence makes this period
especially sensitive and vulnerable in the face of social messages that express
desirability, acceptance, and value in a social group (e.g., Gilbert & Irons, 2009). In this phase of
life, there is also an increased concern regarding social approval, which is
associated with a tendency to focus on competition and self-other evaluations
to gain approval, status, and acceptance from others (Wolfe et al., 1986). These concerns tend to intensify
difficulties with self-presentation, fears of rejection, or disapproval by a
social group, and to be associated with shame (Gilbert & Irons, 2009).
Shame can be
understood as a complex and powerful emotion that can be activated in the face
of social threats, such as the experience of feeling rejected, diminished, ridiculed,
or ostracized by others (e.g., Ferreira
et al., 2020; Gilbert,
1998, 2007; Kaufman, 2004). According to the
evolutionary perspective, shame emerges in the social context as a defensive
response and as a warning sign of the experience of feeling threatened by the
negative evaluation of others (Gilbert,
2002; Gilbert
& Irons, 2009). Thus, shame directs the attentional
focus for social threats (Gilbert,
2002) and usually arouses a set of defensive responses
that aim to correct undesirable perceived personal features or attitudes. This
painful self-conscious emotion has been emphasized as having a significant
impact on people’s sense of self and as a key factor for the development and
maintenance of a wide range of mental health difficulties (e.g., Irons & Gilbert, 2005; Kim et al., 2011; Matos et al., 2013; Pinto-Gouveia & Matos, 2011; Tangney & Dearing, 2002). Although research
on shame feelings and its impact on well-being is scarce in adolescence, there
is evidence that the experience of shame increases from adolescence to middle
adulthood and is negatively associated with psychological well-being (Åslund et al., 2007; Cunha et al., 2012; Orth et al., 2010).
Several accounts have
stressed that individuals with high feelings of shame and self-criticism can
consider compassion and feelings of affiliation and warmth to be aversive or
difficult and may present fears of compassion (i.e., fear of self-directed
compassion, compassion towards others, and receiving compassion from others)
(e.g., Gilbert
et al., 2011, 2012; Longe et al., 2010; Rockliff et al., 2008). Gilbert et al. (2011) conceptualize fears
of compassion as having three dimensions:
Fear of giving and
receiving compassion have consistently been associated with psychopathology
(e.g., insecure attachment; self-criticism; shame; fears of happiness;
depression, anxiety, and stress; Gilbert
et al., 2011, 2012; Kelly et al., 2013), making social
connectedness and social safeness more difficult (Gilbert, 2010). Literature also
shows that in the face of compassionate feelings, early negative backgrounds
are connected to increased vulnerability to the development of aggressive
attitudes and behaviors, fear reactions, resistance, and avoidance (e.g., Gilbert, 2010; Gilbert & Procter, 2006).
On the other hand,
several studies have focused on the benefits of developing compassion and
positive feelings and emotions in adults and adolescents, which seem to promote
psychosocial adjustment, well-being, and social connectedness (Barnard & Curry, 2011; Gilbert & Irons, 2009; Neff & McGehee, 2010). In fact, it has
been argued that the development of compassionate abilities and positive
emotions seem to be promising for the adolescent population (Gilbert & Irons, 2009; Neff & McGehee, 2010). Although poorly
studied in adolescent samples, research conducted with adults demonstrated that
social safeness (i.e., the experience of feeling cared for, reassured and socially
connected and safe) seem to be associated with adaptive strategies to deal more
effectively with negative emotions and with positive affect and well-being (Gilbert, 2005). Further, positive
affect (characterized by feelings of reassurance, warmth, support, and
connectedness in social experiences) has been negatively linked with
psychopathological indicators (e.g., feelings of shame, self-judgment, and
submissiveness; Gilbert et al., 2008;
Kelly et al., 2012).
Therefore, the current
study aimed to examine the relationships between external shame, fear of
receiving compassion, current feelings of social safeness, and adolescents’
psychological well-being. Furthermore, this study also intended to test a path model on a sample of
adolescents of both sexes to explore whether external shame is significantly associated
with their psychological well-being and whether this association is mediated by
fear of receiving compassion and current social feelings of safeness, while
controlling the effect of age. We hypothesized that adolescents with
higher levels of external shame might have lower psychological well-being
indicators, due to increased fear of receiving compassion from others and
decreased feelings of social safeness, support and connectedness.
Participants
A total of 361 adolescents, 158 boys (43.8%)
and 203 girls (56.2%), with a mean age of 14.32 years (SD = 1.47, range
= 12 – 18), participated in the study. These adolescents were from the 7th
to the 11th grade (M = 8.91, SD = 1.26), from middle
and secondary public schools in the district of Coimbra, Portugal. One hundred
eighty-five (51.2%) adolescents were in the early stage of adolescence (ages
between 12 to 14), 170 (47.1%) were in the middle of adolescence (ages range =
15 to 17), and only 6 (1.7%) adolescents were in the late stage of adolescence
(ages from 18 to 21). No significant gender differences were found concerning
age [t(359) = -0.72, p =
.793], and years of education [t(359) = -0.26, p = .494].
Procedures
The sample of the current study was part of the Ph.D. project entitled
“The role of compassionate and competitive mentalities in adolescents’ mental
health: A programme for the promotion of
self-acceptance and self-compassion skills”. This wider research includes
studying the impact of different emotion regulation processes on adolescents’
well-being and mental health. The adolescents’ sample was collected from middle
and secondary public schools in central Portugal. The authorization for the
sample collection was granted by the involved institutions’ Ethics Committees
and Boards (by the Portuguese Ministry of Education, by the Portuguese Data
Protection Authority, and by the Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology
and Education Sciences of the University of Coimbra). The Board of Directors of
Schools and parents were informed about the aims, purpose, and procedures of
the study to give their informed and written consent. Participants were also
informed about the procedures and aims of the study, voluntary participation,
and confidentiality. The adolescents completed the measures in the classroom,
and the researcher was present to assist and clarify instructions or answer
questions about the content of the questionnaires, whenever necessary. Lastly,
to avoid confounding amongst variables, a counterbalanced scale design was
used.
Measures
The Others As Shamer Scale (OAS-2)
The OAS-2 (Portuguese version for adolescents
by Cunha et
al., 2017) consists of eight items
measuring external shame (i.e., the perception that others evaluate and judge
the self negatively). Respondents rated on a five-point Likert scale (0 = “Never”
to 4 = “Almost always”) the frequency of their feelings and experiences. Higher
scores on this measure are indicative of higher external shame. The OAS-2 had a
very good internal consistency, with a Cronbach’s alpha of .82 for the original/adult
version (Matos et al., 2015) and .92 for
the adolescent’s version (Cunha et al., 2017). In the
current study, the scale presented a Cronbach’s alpha value of .91.
Fears of Compassion
Scale in Adolescence (FCS-A)
The FCS-A (Portuguese version by Duarte et al., 2014) is a self-report measure that comprises three subscales: (a) fears of
compassion for self; (b) fears of compassion for others; and (c) fears of
compassion from others, which assesses, specifically, how one reacts to signs
of compassion from other people. Participants are asked to rate each item using
a five point-Likert scale ranging from zero (“Don't agree at all”) to four
(“Completely agree”). In the Portuguese version for adolescents (Duarte et al., 2014), Cronbach's alphas were .93, .86, and .88 for fears of compassion for
self, fears of compassion to others, and fears of receiving compassion from others,
respectively. For the current study, we only used the subscale fear of
receiving compassion from others, which presented a Cronbach alpha of .89.
Social Safeness and
Pleasure Scale (SSPS-A)
The SSPS-A (Portuguese version for adolescents
by Miguel et al., 2019) is an 11-item self-report measure that assesses how
people experience the world as safe, warm, and soothing. It reflects how people
feel in different contexts of social interaction. Each item is rated on a
five-point Likert scale (0 = “Never” to 4 = “Almost always”). Cronbach's alpha
revealed values of .92 and .93 for the original (Gilbert et al., 2009) and the Portuguese versions (Miguel et al., 2019), respectively. In the
current study, the Cronbach's alpha was .94.
Health-Related
Quality of Life Measurement in Children and Adolescents – Short Version
(KIDSCREEN-27)
The KIDSCREEN-27 (Portuguese version by Gaspar & Matos, 2008) contains 27 items divided by five dimensions (Physical Well-Being;
Psychological Well-Being; Autonomy and Parents; Peers and Social Support; and
School Environment). This self-report scale measures the children/adolescents’
perception of their well-being and functioning. The response range is based on
a five-point Likert scale (from 0 = “Never/not at all” to 5 = “Always”). Higher
scores in this measure suggest a better quality of life. The Cronbach's alpha
values were satisfactory across all five dimensions, ranging between .79 for
physical well-being and .84 for psychological well-being (Ravens-Sieberer et al., 2006). According to the current study's purpose, only the psychological
well-being dimension of the KIDSCREEN-27 was used; and presenting a Cronbach's
alpha of .79.
Data
Analysis
The present study had a cross-sectional design and aimed to test the
mediator effect of fear of compassion from others and current feelings of
social safeness between external shame and adolescents’ psychological
well-being. Descriptive statistics were performed to assess sample means and
standard deviations and examine the study variables' associations. To interpret
the effect sizes of Pearson's correlation, Cohen et al. guidelines (2003) were used (i.e., magnitudes between .10 and .30 were considered weak,
.30 to .50 moderate and magnitudes above .50 strong). These data analyses were
performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Science (SSPS, version
22.0; IBM SPSS, Chicago, IL).
Using the Analysis of Moment Structures software (AMOS, version 22.0;
IBM® SPSS® Amos™ 22; Arbuckle,
2013), a series of path analyses
were performed using the Maximum Likelihood method. The significance of direct,
indirect, and total effects was examined through Chi-square tests. The
Bootstrap resampling method, with 5000 samples and 95 percent bias-corrected
confidence intervals, was used to test the mediational paths' significance. The
effect was statistically significant (p < .050) if zero was not included in
the interval between the lower and upper bound of the 95 percent bias-corrected
confidence interval (Kline,
2011). The criteria recommended
for a good model fit were: a non-significant Chi-square (c2; p
> .05); a Comparative Fit Index (CFI) ≥ .95; a
Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) ≥ .95; and a Root Mean Square Error of
Approximation (RMSEA) ≤ .05 (Kline, 2011).
Preliminary
Analyses
Univariate and multivariate analyses were
performed and indicated no severe violations of normal distribution (│Sk│ < 3 and │Ku│< 8 – 10; Kline, 2011). Skewness values ranged from 0.03 (adolescents’ psychological
well-being) to 1.19 (external shame), and with kurtosis values ranging from 0.31
(fear of receiving compassion) to 3.46 (adolescents’ psychological well-being).
Descriptive
Statistics and Differences Between Sexes
Independent t-tests were computed to
analyze group differences in all variables of the study. Means, standard
deviations, t-test differences, and Cohen’s d for all variables
in each group (boys and girls) are displayed in Table 1.
Results suggest that no significant differences were found between boys and
girls in the variables under study.
|
Means, Standard Deviations,
t-Test, and its Significance, and Effect Size for Boys and Girls |
|
||||||||
|
|
Boys (n = 158) |
|
Girls (n = 203) |
t |
p |
Cohen’s d |
|
||
|
|
M |
SD |
|
M |
SD |
|
|||
|
External shame |
7.34 |
6.41 |
|
9.28 |
7.26 |
-1.29 |
n.s. |
0.28 |
|
|
Fears of receiving compassion from others |
18.14 |
10.07 |
|
15.94 |
10.57 |
2.01 |
n.s. |
0.21 |
|
|
Social safeness |
42.40 |
8.56 |
|
42.88 |
8.44 |
-0.53 |
n.s. |
0.08 |
|
|
Adolescents’ psychological well-being |
23.75 |
4.42 |
|
22.83 |
4.90 |
1.84 |
n.s. |
0.20 |
|
|
Note. ns. = non-significant. |
|
Correlations
Correlational analyses are presented for boys
and girls in Table 2. Considering the girls' group, results revealed that
positive correlations, with weak magnitudes, were found between age, external
shame, and fear of compassion from others. However, in the same group, negative
correlations, with small magnitudes, were found between age, current feelings
of social safeness, and adolescents’ psychological well-being. Furthermore, for
both sexes, external shame correlated moderate and positively with fear of
receiving compassion from others and moderate and negatively with current
feelings of social safeness and adolescents’ psychological well-being. In turn,
fear of receiving compassion from others correlated moderate and negatively
with current feelings of social safeness and adolescents’ psychological
well-being. Finally, a correlation between current feelings of social safeness
and adolescents' psychological well-being was significant, positive and strong.
|
Table 2 Intercorrelation Scores on Self-Report Measures for Boys (n = 158;
bottom side of the table) and Girls (n = 203; superior side, in bold) |
|
|||||
|
|
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
|
|
1. Age |
— |
.14* |
.14* |
-.22** |
-.26*** |
|
|
2. External shame |
-.03 |
— |
.41*** |
-.54*** |
-.53*** |
|
|
3. Fears of receiving compassion
from others |
-.13 |
.37*** |
— |
-.39*** |
-.38*** |
|
|
4. Social safeness |
-.01 |
-.58*** |
-.49*** |
— |
.62*** |
|
|
5. Adolescents’ psychological
well-being |
-.12 |
-.58*** |
-.41*** |
.62*** |
— |
|
|
Note. *p
< .05; **p < .01; ***p < .001. |
|
Path
Analysis
A fully saturated model with 20 parameters was
initially examined. All path coefficients were statistically significant at the
level of p < .050. An exception was verified concerning to the path
coefficient between age and fear of receiving compassion from others (bage = -.03; SEb
= 0.35; Z = -0.01; p = .923). This path was removed, and the
model was readjusted.
The readjusted
model is presented in Figure 1
and explained 15.0% of the variance of fear of receiving compassion from
others, 37.0% of the variance of current feelings of social safeness, and 46.0%
of the variance of adolescents’ psychological well-being. Model fit indices
were examined and provided evidence for an excellent fit to empirical data [CMIN/df = .01; TLI = 1.02; CFI = 1.00; NFI = 1.00; RMSEA = .00, p = .951, 95% CI
= .000 to .049].
Findings showed
that external shame had a positive direct effect (β = 0.38) on fear of
receiving compassion from others (bexternal
shame = 0.58; SEb = 0.07; Z = 7.87; p < .001)
and a negative direct effect of -0.44
and -0.29 on feelings of social
safeness (bexternal shame
= -0.54; SEb
= 0.06; Z = -9.73; p
< .001) and adolescents’ psychological well-being (bexternal
shame= -0.20; SEb = 0.03;
Z = -6.07; p < .001), respectively. In turn, fear of receiving
compassion from others presented negative direct effects of -0.26 on feelings
of social safeness (bfear of
receiving compassion= -0.21;
SEb = 0.04; Z = -5.76; p < .001) and of -0.09 on adolescents’ psychological well-being
(bfear of receiving
compassion= -.04; SEb = .02; Z =
-1.98; p < .050). Also, current feelings of social safeness showed a
positive direct effect (β = 0.40) on adolescents’ psychological well-being (bsocial
safeness = 0.22; SEb = 0.03;
Z = 8.31; p < .001).
Regarding indirect
effects, results indicated that external shame presented an indirect effect of
-0.10
on feelings of social safeness, mediated by fear of receiving compassion from
others (95% CI = -0.16 to -0.06). Also,
external shame showed an indirect effect of -0.25 on adolescents’ psychological well-being, through fear of receiving
compassion from others and feelings of social safeness (95% CI
= -0.32 to -0.20). Finally, fear of receiving compassion
also presented an indirect effect of -0.11 on adolescents’ psychological well-being, via feelings of social
safeness (95% CI = -0.16 to -0.06). These
results seem to suggest that external shame may be associated with the
adolescents’ perception of decreased psychological well-being through higher
levels of fear of receiving compassion from others and lower current feelings
of social safeness.
Moreover, to
examine if there was invariance of the model between the groups (boys and
girls), a multi-group comparison was explored. It showed non-significant
differences in the path coefficients from the final model between the two
groups.
Figure 1
Final path
model. Standardized Path Coefficients among Variables are Presented (click here to
view).
Several studies
have emphasized the association between feelings of shame (i.e., feelings that
are activated in the face of social threats, involving experiences of
inferiority) and the development and maintenance of a variety of mental health
problems in adults (e.g., Irons & Gilbert, 2005; Kim et al., 2011; Matos et al., 2013; Pinto-Gouveia & Matos, 2011; Tangney & Dearing, 2002). However, the relationship between external
shame and psychological well-being and the mechanisms which may explain this
association has been left unexplored in adolescents.
Findings of
correlation analyses seem to support our initial predictions. They are in line
with previous literature demonstrating that external shame was significantly
and negatively associated with adolescents’ psychological well-being (Mendes et al., 2019). Furthermore, correlation results seem to indicate that external
shame was positively associated with the fear of receiving compassion from
others and negatively with feelings of social safeness, support, and
connectedness. These emotion regulation processes are, in turn, negatively
linked to each other and are significantly associated with adolescents’
psychological well-being. Additionally, it was interesting to note that age
presented a significant and negative association with adolescents’
psychological well-being. In fact, these findings follow previous research,
which recognized that life satisfaction and quality of life decrease during
adolescence (e.g., Bisegger et al.,
2005;
Goldbeck et al., 2007). Considering this data, the effect of age
was controlled in the model.
To better clarify
the associations between the study variables, the current model was explored
via path analyses. The model revealed an excellent fit to empirical data,
accounting for 46.0% of adolescents’ psychological well-being variance. Results
seemed to demonstrate that although external shame has a direct effect on
adolescents’ psychological well-being, this association is also mediated by
fears of receiving compassion from others and difficulties of establishing a
secure and connected relationship with them. Our analyses showed that feelings
of external shame directly explained 15.0% of the variance of fears of receiving
compassion, and 37.0% of the variance of current feelings of safeness and
connectedness was directly explained by external shame and indirectly through
fears of receiving compassion. Further,
results seem to indicate that the tested model is invariant for boys and girls,
and there are no differences in the relationship between the studied variables.
These findings are in line with our hypothesis, suggesting that, in the adolescents’
population, despite external shame is directly associated with lower levels of
psychological well-being, this relationship is also indirectly carried by
emotional regulation processes (Mendes et al., 2019), such as fear of receiving compassion from
others and social safeness.
Thus, considering that external shame is based on
the perception of oneself as inferior in others' minds, it is important to
maintain safe social relationships. In this critical phase of adolescence,
relationships with peers are the first to be established outside of a family
context, and so they will become a reference for future relationships. If
adolescents already have feelings of shame, they must develop relationships
based on quality, care, and safety to increase their psychological well-being.
In the association between external shame and psychological well-being, the
fear of receiving compassion from others seems to have an important role. In
this way, if one feels inferior in the mind of others, the fear of receiving
compassion from them (many times viewed as a sign of weakness and
vulnerability) could reinforce the sense of inferiority and shame, consequently
having an impact on the adolescents’ perception of their psychological
well-being.
The current study
presents some limitations that should be mentioned. Firstly, these findings are
based on a cross-sectional design study, and therefore conclusions of causal
relationships between the variables should be prevented. Future studies should
explore if external shame effectively impacts adolescents’ psychological
well-being through a longitudinal design and explore whether fear of receiving
compassion from others and current feelings of social safeness and
connectedness may interfere in this association over time.
Secondly, the
present study explored a model that addresses the impact of specific variables
(e.g., external shame, fears of receiving compassion, and current feelings of
social safeness) in adolescents’ quality of life. Therefore, other relevant
variables were not considered (such as mechanisms associated with psychological
inflexibility). Hence, future studies should examine the role of these
variables.
Another limitation
is that the questionnaire protocol was exclusively composed of self-report
measures, which may present some bias (e.g., social desirability) that may
compromise the data's validity. Future research should consider using other
assessment methodologies (e.g., structured interviews, and other sources of
information, i.e., parents, teachers, peer group, or other significant
figures). Lastly, participants were recruited in school units only from central
Portugal, which may compromise the results' generalization to other samples.
However, these schools were selected to encompass urban and rural backgrounds
and to examine various socioeconomic contexts.
Despite
limitations, the model examined in the present study may have relevant
implications for future research and consider when developing intervention
programs that promote quality of life and well-being in adolescents. Since
adolescence represents a critical developmental stage in which a series of
psychosocial changes that may have a negative impact on mental health and
psychosocial functioning occur (e.g., Gilbert & Irons, 2009; Irons & Gilbert, 2005), the study of these associations and the
mechanisms that contribute to enhancing psychological well-being come out as
particularly relevant.
This study seems to contribute towards a
better understanding of the important role that feelings of inferiority, fears
of receiving compassion from others and difficulty of establishing safe and
pleasurable relationships with others have on the psychological well-being of
adolescents of both sexes. Indeed, several studies in adult samples have suggested that
compassion skills are positively associated with well-being indicators, such as
social connectedness, positive affect, and happiness, and negatively linked
with shame, self-criticism, rumination, and depression (e.g., Barnard
& Curry, 2011; Goetz et al., 2010; Neff, 2003; Neff et al., 2007). Furthermore, several studies suggest that
the development of compassion skills have a powerful effect on psychological
and physical well-being (e.g., Hall et al., 2013; Jazaieri et al.,
2014;
Neff et al., 2007; Neff & McGehee, 2010; Pace et al., 2008). Considering its benefits, the promotion of
these compassionate attitudes, both towards themselves and towards others,
become the focus of training programs and clinical interventions for mental
health problems (e.g., individuals with high levels of self-criticism and
shame; Gilbert & Procter, 2006). Although self-compassion has been
identified as a factor of well-being in adolescents, functioning as a “buffer”
against a wide range of negative psychological health outcomes (Marsh et al., 2018), the study of emotional mechanisms that have an impact on
adolescents’ well-being has been scarcely investigated.
Our study findings
support the relevance of working with adolescents with high levels of shame,
fear of compassion, and low feelings of security and connection to a social
group towards more compassionate ways of functioning. They seem to corroborate
the importance of helping them develop alternative and adaptive emotion
regulation processes and mechanisms.
Arbuckle, J. L. (2013). Amos (Version
22.0) [Computer Program]. SPSS.
Åslund, C., Nilsson, K. W., Starrin,
B., & Sjöberg, R. L. (2007). Shaming experiences
and the association between adolescent depression and psychosocial risk
factors. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 16, 298–304. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-006-0564-1
Barnard, L. K.,
& Curry, J. F. (2011). Self-compassion: Conceptualizations, correlates,
& interventions. Review of General Psychology, 15(4),
289–303. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025754
Bisegger,
C., Cloetta, B., Rueden, U., Abel, T., Ravens-Sieberer, & European KIDSCREEN Group. (2005).
Health-related quality of life: Gender differences in childhood and adolescence.
Schweizerische Gesellschaft für
Sozial- und Präventivmedizin,
50(5), 281–291. https://doi.org/ 10.1007/s00038-005-4094-2
Cohen, J., Cohen,
P., West, S. G., & Aiken, L. S. (2003). Applied multiple
regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (3rd ed.).
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
Cunha, M., Matos,
M., Faria, D., & Zagalo,
S. (2012). Shame memories and psychopathology in adolescence: The mediator
effect of shame. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological
Therapy, 12(2), 203–218. https://bit.ly/3f3tqcE
Cunha, M., Xavier,
A., Cherpe, S., & Pinto-Gouveia, J. (2017).
Assessment of shame in adolescents: ‘The Other As
Shamer Scale’. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 33, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1590/0102.3772e3336
Duarte, C., Pinto-Gouveia, J., & Cunha, M.
(2014). Fears of compassion in adolescence: Fears of
Compassion Scales’ examination and implications in this population [Poster presentation]. The 3rd International
Conference on Compassion Focused Therapy. Birmingham, UK.
Ferreira, C., Moura-Ramos,
M., Matos, M., & Galhardo, A. (2020). A new measure to assess external and internal
shame: Development, factor structure and psychometric properties of the
external and internal shame scale. Current Psychology, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-020-00709-0
Gaspar, T., &
Matos, M. G. (2008). Qualidade de vida em crianças
e adolescentes: Versão portuguesa dos instrumentos
KIDSCREEN-52 [Quality of life in children and adolescents: Portuguese
version of the KIDSCREEN-52 instruments]. Aventura Social e Saúde.
Gaspar, T., Ribeiro, J. P., de Matos, M. G., Leal,
I., & Ferreira, A. (2012). Health-related
quality of life in children and adolescents: subjective well
being. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 15(1),
177–186. https://doi.org/10.5209/rev_SJOP.2012.v15.n1.37306
Gilbert, P. (1998).
What is shame? Some core issues and controversies. Em
P. Gilbert & B. Andrews (Eds.), Series in affective science. Shame:
Interpersonal behavior, psychopathology, and culture (pp. 3–38). Oxford
University Press.
Gilbert, P. (2002). Body shame: A biopsychosocial conceptualization and
overview, with treatment implications. Em P. Gilbert
& Miles (Eds.), Body shame: Conceptualisation,
research and treatment (pp. 3–54). Brunner Routledge.
Gilbert, P. (2005).
Compassion and cruelty: A biopsychosocial approach. Em P.
Gilbert (Ed.), Compassion: Conceptualisations,
research and use in psychotherapy (pp. 9–74). Routledge.
Gilbert, P. (2007).
The evolution of shame as a marker for relationship security. Em J. L. Tracy, R. W. Robins, & J. P. Tangney (Eds.), The
self-conscious emotions: Theory and research (pp. 283–309). Guilford Press.
Gilbert, P.
(2010). The CBT distinctive features series. Compassion focused
therapy: Distinctive features. Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
Gilbert, P., &
Irons, C. (2009). Shame, self-criticism, and self-compassion in
adolescence. Em N. B. Allen & l. B. Sheeber (Eds.), Adolescent emotional development and the
emergence of depressive disorders (pp. 195–214). Cambridge University
Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9780511551963.011
Gilbert, P., McEwan, K., Gibbons, L., Chotai, S., Duarte, J., & Matos, M. (2012). Fears of compassion and happiness in relation
to alexithymia, mindfulness, and self-criticism. Psychology and
Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 85(4), 374–390. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8341.2011.02046.x
Gilbert, P.,
McEwan, K., Matos, M., & Rivis, A. (2011). Fears
of compassion: Development of three self‐report measures. Psychology
and Psychotherapy: Theory, Research and Practice, 84(3), 239–255.
https://doi.org/10.1348/147608310X526511
Gilbert, P.,
McEwan, K., Mitra, R., Franks, L., Richter, A., & Rockliff, H. (2008).
Feeling safe and content: A specific affect regulation system? Relationship to
depression, anxiety, stress, and self-criticism. The Journal of
Positive Psychology, 3(3), 182–191. https://doi.org/10.1080/17439760801999461
Gilbert, P.,
McEwan, K., Mitra, R., Richter, A., Franks, L., Mills, A., & Bellew, R.,
& Gale, C. (2009). An exploration of different types of positive affect in
students and in patients with bipolar disorder. Clinical Neuropsychiatry:
Journal of Treatment Evaluation, 6(4), 135–143. https://bit.ly/3nsNsAe
Gilbert,
P., & Procter, S. (2006). Compassionate mind training for people with high
shame and self-criticism: overview and pilot study of a group therapy approach.
Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 13(6), 353–379.
https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.507
Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D., & Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion:
An evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(3),
351–374. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018807
Goldbeck, L., Schmitz, T. G., Besier, T., Herschbach,
P., & Henrich, G. (2007). Life satisfaction decreases during
adolescence. Quality of Life Research, 16(6), 969–979.
https://www.jstor.org/stable/27641333
Hall,
C. W., Row, K. A., Wuensch, K. L., & Godley, K.
R. (2013). The role of self-compassion in physical and psychological
well-being. The Journal of Psychology, 147(4), 311–323.
https://doi.org/10.1080/00223980.2012.693138
Irons, C., & Gilbert, P. (2005). Evolved
mechanisms in adolescent anxiety and depression symptoms: The role of the
attachment and social rank systems. Journal of Adolescence, 28(3),
325–341. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.adolescence.2004.07.004
Jazaieri,
H., McGonigal, K., Jinpa, T., Doty, J. R., Gross, J.
J., & Goldin, P. R. (2014). A randomized controlled trial of compassion
cultivation training: Effects on mindfulness, affect, and emotion regulation. Motivation
and Emotion, 38(1), 23–35. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-013-9368-z
Kaufman, G.
(2004). The psychology of shame: Theory and treatment of shame-based
syndromes. Springer Publishing Company.
Kelly, A. C.,
Carter, J. C., Zuroff, D. C., & Borairi, S. (2013). Self-compassion and fear of
self-compassion interact to predict response to eating disorders treatment: A
preliminary investigation. Psychotherapy Research, 23(3),
252–264. https://doi.org/10.1080/10503307.2012.717310
Kelly, A. C., Zuroff, D. C., Leybman, M. J.,
& Gilbert, P. (2012). Social safeness received social support, and
maladjustment: Testing a tripartite model of affect regulation. Cognitive
Therapy and Research, 36(6), 815–826. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9432-5
Kim, S., Thibodeau,
R., & Jorgensen, R. S. (2011). Shame, guilt, and depressive symptoms: A
meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 137(1),
68–96. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021466
Kline, R. B.
(2011). Principles and practice of structural equation modelling (3rd
ed.). Guilford Press.
Longe, O., Maratos, F.
A., Gilbert, P., Evans, G., Volker, F., Rockliff, H., & Rippon, G. (2010).
Having a word with yourself: Neural correlates of self-criticism and
self-reassurance. NeuroImage, 49(2),
1849–1856. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.019
Marsh, I. C., Chan,
S. W., & MacBeth, A. (2018). Self-compassion and
psychological distress in adolescents — A meta-analysis. Mindfulness, 9(4), 1011–1027. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0850-7
Matos, M., Pinto-Gouveia, J., Gilbert, P., Duarte,
C., & Figueiredo, C. (2015). The
Other As Shamer Scale–2: Development and validation of
a short version of a measure of external shame. Personality and
Individual Differences, 74, 6–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.09.037
Matos, M.,
Pinto‐Gouveia, J., & Gilbert, P. (2013). The effect of shame and shame
memories on paranoid ideation and social anxiety. Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 20(4), 334–349. https://doi.org/10.1002/cpp.1766
Mendes, A. L., Canavarro, C.,
& Ferreira, C. (2019). The
link between the recall of early emotional experiences and well-being in
adolescence: The roles of shame and of a self-critical attitude [Manuscript
submitted for publication]. Faculdade de Psicologia e Ciências da Educação, Universidade de
Coimbra.
Miguel, R. R., Sousa, R., Brazão,
N., Rijo, D., Castilho, P., & Gilbert, P. (2019).
Psychometric properties and measurement invariance of the Social Safeness and
Pleasure Scale in adolescent samples [Manuscript submitted for in preparation].
Faculdade de Psicologia e Ciências da Educação, Universidade de Coimbra.
Neff, K. D. (2003).
Self-compassion: An alternative conceptualization of a healthy attitude toward
oneself. Self and Identity, 2(2), 85–101.
https://doi.org/10.1080/15298860309032
Neff, K. D., &
McGehee, P. (2010). Self-compassion and psychological resilience among
adolescents and young adults. Self and Identity, 9(3),
225–240. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298860902979307
Neff,
K. D., Kirkpatrick, K. L., & Rude, S. S. (2007). Self-compassion and
adaptive psychological functioning. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1),
139–154. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.03.004
Orth, U., Robins, R. W., & Soto, C. J.
(2010). Tracking the trajectory of shame, guilt, and pride across the life
span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(6), 1061–1071. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021342
Pace, T. W., Negi,
L. T., Adame, D. D., Cole, S. P., Sivilli,
T. I., Brown, T. D., Issa, M. J., & Raison, C. L. (2009). Effect of
compassion meditation on neuroendocrine, innate immune and behavioral responses
to psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology,
34(1), 87-98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.08.011
Pinto-Gouveia, J., & Matos, M. (2011). Can shame memories become a key to identity?
The centrality of shame memories predicts psychopathology. Applied
Cognitive Psychology, 25(2), 281–290. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.1689
Proctor, C. L.,
Linley, P. A., & Maltby, J. (2009). Youth life satisfaction: A review of
the literature. Journal of Happiness Studies, 10(5),
583–630. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-008-9110-9
Ravens-Sieberer, U., Gosch, A., Erhart, M., von Rueden, U., Nickel,
J., Kurth, B.-M., Duer, W.,
Fuerth, K., Czemy, L., Auquier, P., Simeoni, M.-C., Robitail, S., Tountas, Y., Dimitrakaki, C., Czimbalmos, A., Aszmann, A., Kilroe, J., Keenaghan, C.,
Bruil, J., … The KIDSCREEN Group Europe. (2006). The
KIDSCREEN questionnaires. Quality of life questionnaires for children and
adolescents. Pabst Science Publishers.
Salmela-Aro, K., & Tuominen-Soini, H. (2010).
Adolescents’ life satisfaction during the transition to post-comprehensive
education: Antecedents and consequences. Journal of Happiness Studies, 11(6),
683–701. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10902-009-9156-3
Tangney, J. P.,
& Dearing, R. L. (2002). Emotions and social behavior. Shame and
guilt. Em D. Levinson (Ed.), Encyclopedia of
crime and punishment (Vol. 1, pp. 1492-1494). SAGE Publications. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781412950664.n388
Wolfe, R. N.,
Lennox, R. D., & Cutler, B. L. (1986). Getting along and getting ahead:
Empirical support for a theory of protective and acquisitive self-presentation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 356–361.
https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.356
World Health
Organization. (2020). Adolescent health and development. https://www.who.int/news-room/q-a-detail/adolescent-health-and-development
Publicação em
Acesso Aberto © 2020. O(s) Autor(es). Este é um artigo de acesso aberto distribuído
sob a Licença Creative Commons Attribution, que
permite uso, distribuição e reprodução sem restrições em qualquer meio, desde
que o trabalho original seja devidamente citado. |
Open Access Publication © 2020. The Author(s).
This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons
Attribution License,
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided
the
original work is properly cited. |