Memory and aging: What is the real impact of age?
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.7342/ismt.rpics.2016.2.2.40Keywords:
Aging, Memory, Institutionalization, Sociodemographic characteristicsAbstract
Background & Goals: Memory impairment is one of the types of cognitive impairment that most affects the elderly. Age is considered one of the major factors in memory impairment, including by the elderly themselves. Research has shown that there are other factors affecting the memory of elderly persons. It remains, however, unclear what is the real impact of age in memory when controlling the influence of other variables. Thus, this study aims to analyze the impact of age on memory functioning of elderly persons and check if the potential impact remains when controlling the role of other variables (sex, education, profession, marital status, residential status, and clinical situation). Method: The global sample comprised 1126 subjects (283 men and 843 women, 226 residents in the community, and 900 institutionalized elderly) aged from 60 to 100 years. The assessment included items from the Mini-Mental State Examination (working memory), the Montreal Cognitive Assessment factor (verbal declarative memory), and Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure (visuospatial memory). Results: Overall, age, education, profession, marital, residential, and clinical condition have differently influenced memory, depending on the type of memory. The hierarchical regression analysis showed that age is a predictive factor in all types of memory. However, other predictors have emerged with higher regression coefficients compared to age, according to the type of memory (except in working memory). Conclusions: Age, education, and profession influence memory, as well as factors that potentially stimulate cognitively and socially (like having a partner and living in the community). The results indicate the importance of intervening, especially among institutionalized elderly, older, unmarried, with low education, and manual profession.
Downloads
References
Bäckman, L., Small, B. J., Wahlin, Å., & Larsson, M. (2000). Cognitive functioning in very old age. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A. Salthouse (Eds.), The handbook of aging and cognition (2nd ed., pp. 499-558). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
Baldivia, B., Andrade, V. M., & Bueno, O. F. A. B. (2008). Contribution of education, occupation and cognitively stimulating activities to the formation of cognitive reserve. Dementia & Neuropsychologia, 2(3), 173-182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Bangen, K. J., Jack, A. J., Schiehser, D. M., Delano-Wood, L., Tuminello, E., Han, S. D., . . . Bondi, M. W. (2010). Complex activities of daily living vary by mild cognitive impairment subtype. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(4), 630-639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Bartrés-Faz, D., Junqué, C., López-Alomar, A., Valveny, N., Moral, P., Casamayor, R., . . . Clemente, I. C. (2001). Neuropsychological and genetic differences between age-associated memory impairment and mild cognitive impairment entities. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 49(7), 985-990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Bennett, D. A., Schneider, J. A., Buchman, A. S., Barnes, L. L., Boyle, P. A., & Wilson, R. S. (2012). Overview and findings from the Rush Memory and Aging Project. Current Alzheimer Research, 9(6), 646-663. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Bickel, H., & Cooper, B. (1994). Incidence and relative risk of dementia in an urban elderly population: Findings of a prospective field-study. Psychological Medicine, 24(1), 179-192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Boone, K. B., Lesser, I. M., Hill-Gutierrez, E., Berman, N. G., & D’Elia, L. F. (1993). Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure performance in healthy, older adults: Relationship to age education, sex and IQ. Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7(1), 22-28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Buchner, A., Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., & Lang, A. G. (2014). G*Power 3 (Version 3.1.9.2 for Macintosh) [Computer software]. Düsseldorf: Heinrich-Heine-Universität Dusseldorf [Google Scholar] [URL]
Casanova-Sotolongo, P., Casanova-Carrillo, P., & Casanova-Carrillo, C. (2004). La memoria. Introducción al estudio de los trastornos cognitivos en el envejecimiento normal y patológico [Memory. An introduction to the study of the cognitive disorders in normal and pathological aging. Revista de Neurología, 38(5), 469-472. [Google Scholar] [URL]
Chang, A. (2013). StatsToDo (Sample size for estimating a single alpha program) (online calculator). Brisbane, Queensland, Austrália. [Google Scholar] [URL]
Chen, T., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2012). Assessing the associative deficit of older adults in long-term and short-term/working memory. Psychology and Aging, 27(3), 666-682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Cherkin, A. (1968). Kinetics of memory consolidation: Role of amnesic treatment parameters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United. States of America, 63(4), 1094-1101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Comijs, H. C., Kriegsman, D. M. W., Dik, M. G., Deeg, D. J. H., Jonker, C., & Stalman, W. A. B. (2009). Somatic chronic diseases and 6-year change in cognitive functioning among older persons. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 48(2), 10-1016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Conroy, R. M., Golden, J., Jeffares, I., O' Neill, D., & McGee, H. (2010). Boredom-proneness, loneliness, social engagement and depression and their association with cognitive function in older people: A population study. Psychology, Health & Medicine, 15(4), 463-473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Cowan, N., Elliott, E. M., Scott Saults, J., Morey, C. C., Mattox, S., Hismjatullina, A., & Conway, A. R. A. (2005). On the capacity of attention: Its estimation and its role in working memory and cognitive aptitudes. Cognitive Psychology, 51(1), 42-100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Craik, F. I. M. (1994). Memory changes in normal aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3(5), 155-158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Crook, T., Bartus, R. T., Ferris, S. H., Whitehouse, P., Cohen, G. D., & Gershon, S. (1986). Age‐associated memory impairment: Proposed diagnostic criteria and measures of clinical change — report of a national institute of mental health work group. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2(4), 261-276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Dansilio, S., & Charamelo, A. (2005). Constructional functions and figure copying in illiterates or low-schooled Hispanics. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20(8), 1105-1112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Delis, D. C., & Kramer, J. H. (2000). Advances in neuropsychological assessment of memory disorders. In F. Boller & J. Grafman (Eds.), Handbook of neuropsychology (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 25-47). Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers. [Google Scholar]
Duro, D., Simões, M. R., Ponciano, E., & Santana, I. (2010). Validation studies of the Portuguese experimental version of the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA): Confirmatory factor analysis. Journal of Neurology, 257(5), 728-734. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Elias, M. F., Elias, P. K., D’Agostino, R. B., Silbershatz, H., & Wolf, P. A. (1997). Role of age, education, and gender on cognitive performance in the Framingham Heart Study: Community-based norms. Experimental Aging Research, 23(3), 201-235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Espirito-Santo, H., & Daniel, F. (2015). Calcular e apresentar tamanhos do efeito em trabalhos científicos (1): As limitações do p < 0,05 na análise de diferenças de médias de dois grupos - Folha de cálculo suplementar [Calculating and reporting effect sizes on scientific papers (1): p < 0.05 limitations in the analysis of mean differences of two groups – Supplemental worksheet]. Revista Portuguesa de Investigação Comportamental e Social, 1(1), 3-16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Fastame, M. C., Hitchcott, P. K., Penna, M. P., & Murino, G. (2016). Does institutionalization influence perceived metamemory, psychological well-being, and working-memory efficiency in Italian elders? A preliminary study. Journal of Clinical Gerontology and Geriatrics, 7(1), 6-11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189-198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Fratiglioni, L., Wang, H. X., Ericsson, K., Maytan, M., & Winblad, B. (2000). Influence of social network on occurrence of dementia: A community-based longitudinal study. The Lancet, 355, 1315-1319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Gallagher, C., & Burke, T. (2007). Age, gender and IQ effects on the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 46(1), 35-45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Ghisletta, P., Bickel, J. F., & Lövdén, M. (2006). Does activity engagement protect against cognitive decline in old age? Methodological and analytical considerations. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 61(5), 253-261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Giogkaraki, E., Michaelides, M. P., & Constantinidou, F. (2013). The role of cognitive reserve in cognitive aging: Results from the neurocognitive study on aging. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 35(10), 1024-1035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Gold, P. E., & McGaugh, J. L. (1975). A single-trace, two-process view of memory storage processes. In D. Deutsch & J. A. Deutsch (Eds.), Short-term memory (pp. 355–378). New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
Gordo, S. M. O. (2015). Estudio del funcionamiento ejecutivo y de las actividades de la vida diaria en ancianos en función del ámbito de residencia habitual [Study of executive functioning and activities of daily life in older people according to the context of habitual residence] (Doctoral dissertation). [Google Scholar] [Handle]
Guerreiro, M. (2010). Testes de rastreio de defeito cognitivo e demência: Uma perspectiva prática [Screening tests for cognitive impairment and dementia: A practical perspetive]. Revista Portuguesa de Clínica Geral, 26(1), 46-53. [Google Scholar] [URL]
Guerreiro, M., Silva, A. P., Botelho, M. A., Leitão, O., Castro-Caldas, A., & Garcia, C. (1994). Adaptação à população portuguesa da tradução do “Mini-Mental State Examination” (MMSE) [Adaptation to the Portuguese population of the translation of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)]. Revista Portuguesa de Neurologia, 1, 9-10. [Google Scholar]
Håkansson, K., Rovio, S., Helkala, E. L., Vilska, A. R., Winblad, B., Soininen, H., . . . Kivipelto, M. (2009). Association between mid-life marital status and cognitive function in later life: Population based cohort study. BMJ, 339(99), 1-8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Hänninen, T., Hallikainen, M., Koivisto, K., Helkala, E. L., Reinikainen, K. J., Soininen, H., . . . Riekkinen, P. J. Sr. (1995). A follow‐up study of age‐associated memory impairment: Neuropsychological predictors of dementia. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 43(9), 1007-1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Hassing, L. B., Hofer, S. M., Nilsson, S. E., Berg, S., Pedersen, N. L., McClearn, G., & Johansson, B. (2004). Comorbid type 2 diabetes mellitus and hypertension exacerbates cognitive decline: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Age and Ageing, 33(4), 355-361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Herlitz, A., & Rehnman, J. (2008). Sex differences in episodic memory. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(1), 52-56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Herlitz, A., Nilsson, L. G., & Bäckman, L. (1997). Gender differences in episodic memory. Memory & Cognition, 25(6), 801-811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Hertzog, C., Kramer, A. F., Wilson, R. S., & Lindenberger, U. (2008). Enrichment effects on adult cognitive development: Can the functional capacity of older adults be preserved and enhanced. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 9(1), 1-65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Howieson, D. B., Holm, L. A., Kaye, J. A., Oken, B. S., & Howieson, J. (1993). Neurologic function in the optimally healthy oldest old: Neuropsychological evaluation. Neurology, 43(10), 1882-1886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J., & Dixon, R. A. (1999). Use it or lose it: Engaged lifestyle as a buffer of cognitive decline in aging? Psychology and Aging, 14(2), 245-263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Hultsch, D. F., Hertzog, C., Small, B. J., McDonald-Miszczak, L., & Dixon, R. A. (1992). Short-term longitudinal change in cognitive performance in later life. Psychology and Aging, 7(4), 571-584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Huppert, F., Johnson, T., & Nickson, J. (2000). High prevalence of prospective memory impairment in the elderly and in early-stage dementia: Findings from a population-based study. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 14(7), 63-81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Jonker, C., Geerlings, M. I., & Schmand, B. (2000). Are memory complaints predictive for dementia? A review of clinical and population-based studies. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 15(11), 983-991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Kim, H. Y. (2013). Statistical notes for clinical researchers: Assessing normal distribution (2) using skewness and kurtosis. Restorative Dentistry & Endodontics, 38(1), 52-54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Koivisto, K., Reinikainen, K. J., Hänninen, T., Vanhanen, M., Helkala, E. L., Mykkänen, L., . . . Riekkinen, P. J. Sr. (1995). Prevalence of age-associated memory impairment in a randomly selected population from eastern Finland. Neurology, 45(4), 741-747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Kral, V. A. (1962). Senescent forgetfulness: Benign and malignant. Canadian Medical Association Journal, 86(6), 257-260. [Google Scholar] [PMC]
Larrabee, G. J., Levin, H. S., & High, W. M. (1986). Senescent forgetfulness: A quantitative study. Developmental Neuropsychology, 2(4), 373-385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Biegler, E. D., & Tranel, D. (2012). Neuropsychological assessment (5th ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
Lobo, A., Saz, P., Marcos, G., Día, J. L., de la Cámara, C., Ventura, T., . . . Aznar, S. (1999). Revalidation and standardization of the cognition mini-exam (first Spanish version of the Mini-Mental Status Examination) in the general geriatric population. Medicina Clínica, 112(20), 767-774. [Google Scholar]
Lövdén, M., Ghisletta, P., & Lindenberger, U. (2005). Social participation attenuates decline in perceptual speed in old and very old age. Psychology and Aging, 20(3), 423-434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Lövdén, M., Rönnlund, M., Wahlin, Å., Bäckman, L., Nyberg, L., & Nilsson, L. G. (2004). The extent of stability and change in episodic and semantic memory in old age: Demographic predictors of level and change. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 59(3), 130-134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Maroof, D. A. (2012). Statistical methods in neuropsychology: Common procedures made comprehensible. Boston, MA: Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
May, A. (2011). Experience-dependent structural plasticity in the adult human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(10), 475-482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Maylor, E. A., Reimers, S., Choi, J., Collaer, M. L., Peters, M., & Silverman, I. (2007). Gender and sexual orientation differences in cognition across adulthood: Age is kinder to women than to men regardless of sexual orientation. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 36(2), 235-249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Mega, M. S., Small, G. W., Xu, M. L., Felix, J., Manese, M., Tran, N. P., . . . Toga, A. W. (2002). Hippocampal atrophy in persons with age-associated memory impairment: Volumetry within a common space. Psychosomatic Medicine, 64(3), 487-492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Meinz, E. J., & Salthouse, T. A. (1998). Is age kinder to females than to males. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 5(3), 56-70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Meyers, J. E., & Meyers, K. R. (1995). Rey Complex Figure Test and Recognition Trial: Professional manual. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources. [Google Scholar]
Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., . . . Chertkow, H. (2005). The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53(4), 695-699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Naveh-Benjamin, M., Maddox, G. B., Jones, P., Old, S., & Kilb, A. (2012). The effects of emotional arousal and gender on the associative memory deficit of older adults. Memory & Cognition, 40(4), 551-566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Nielsen, H., Lolk, A., & Kragh-Sørensen, P. (1998). Age-associated memory impairment--pathological memory decline or normal aging? Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 39(1), 33-37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Nyberg, L., Maitland, S. B., Rönnlund, M., Bäckman, L., Dixon, R. A., Wahlin, Å., & Nilsson, L. G. (2003). Selective adult age differences in an age-invariant multifactor model of declarative memory. Psychology and Aging, 18(1), 149-160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Old, S., & Naveh-Benjamin, M. (2008). Memory for people and their actions: Further evidence for an age-related associative deficit. Psychology and Aging, 23(2), 467-472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Osterrieth, P. A. (1944). Le test de copie d’une figure complexe, contribution à l’étude de la perception et de la mémoire [Test of copying a complex figure; contribution to the study of perception and memory]. Archives de Psychologie, 30, 206-356. [Google Scholar]
Pallant, J. (2011). SPSS Survival Manual: A step by step guide to data analysis using SPSS for Windows (4th ed.). Crows Nest: Allen & Unwin. [Google Scholar]
Park, D. C., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. (2009). The adaptive brain: Aging and neurocognitive scaffolding. Annual Review of Psychology, 60(1), 173-196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N. S., Smith, A. D., & Smith, P. K. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17(2), 299-320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Parnetti, L., Lowenthal, D. T., Presciutti, O., Pelliccioli, G. P., Palumbo, R., Gobbi, G., Chiarini, P., Palumbo, B., Tarducci, R., Senin, U. (1996). 1H-MRS, MRI-based hippocampal volumetry, and 99mTc-HMPAO-SPECT in normal aging, age-associated memory impairment, and probable Alzheimer's Disease. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 44(2), 133-138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Pascual-Millán, L. F., Fernández, T., Saz, P., Lobo, A., & Morales-Asín, F. (2000). Exploración de la memoria de trabajo con el miniexamen cognoscitivo [Working memory assessment with mini-mental state examination]. Revista de Neurologia, 30(1), 1-4. [Google Scholar] [URL]
Pauls, F., Petermann, F., & Lepach, A. C. (2013). Gender differences in episodic memory and visual working memory including the effects of age. Memory, 21(7), 857-874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Pena, I. T. (2014). Funcionamiento neuropsicológico y emocional en el envejecimiento. Estudio comparativo entre ancianos institucionalizados y no institucionalizados [Neuropsychological and emotional functioning in aging. Comparative study between institutionalized and non-institutionalized older people] (Doctoral dissertation). [Google Scholar] [Handle]
Peterson, R. A. (1994). A meta-analysis of Cronbach's coefficient alpha. Journal of Consumer Research, 21(2), 381-391. [Google Scholar] [JSTOR]
Plassman. B. L., Williams, J. W., Burke, J. R., Holsinger, T., & Benjamin, S. (2010). Systematic review: Factors associated with risk for and possible prevention of cognitive decline in later life. Annals of Internal Medicine, 153(3), 182-193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Potter, G. G., Helms, M. J., & Plassman, B. L. (2008). Associations of job demands and intelligence with cognitive performance among men in late life. Neurology, 70(19, Part 2), 1803-1808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Reed, B. R., Mungas, D., Farias, S. T., Harvey, D., Beckett, L., Widaman, K., . . . DeCarli, C. (2010). Measuring cognitive reserve based on the decomposition of episodic memory variance. Brain, 133(8), 2196-2209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Rey, A. (1941). L’examen psychologique dans les cas d’encéphalopathie traumatique. (Les problems) [The psychological examination in cases of traumatic encepholopathy Problems]. Archives de Psychologie, 28, 215-285. [Google Scholar]
Rey, A. (2002). Rey: Teste de cópia de figuras complexas [Rey: Complex figures copy test] (2nd ed.). Lisboa: CEGOC-TEA. [Google Scholar]
Rönnlund, M., Nyberg, L., Bäckman, L., & Nilsson, L. G. (2005). Stability, growth, and decline in adult life span development of declarative memory: Cross-sectional and longitudinal data from a population-based study. Psychology and Aging, 20(1), 3-18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Rosselli, M., & Ardila, A. (1991). Effect of age, education, and gender on the Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure. Clinical Neuropsychology, 5(4), 370-376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Rosselli, M., & Ardila, A. (2003). The impact of culture and education on non-verbal neuropsychological measurements: A critical review. Brain and Cognition, 52(3), 326-333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Rubin, E. H., Storandt, M., Miller, J. P., Kinscherf, D. A., Grant, E. A., Morris, J. C., & Berg, L. (1998). A prospective study of cognitive function and onset of dementia in cognitively healthy elders. Archives of Neurology, 55(3), 395-401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Salthouse, T. A. (2003). Interrelations of aging, knowledge, and cognitive performance. In U. M. Staudinger & U. Lindenberger (Eds.), Understanding human development: Dialogues with lifespan psychology (pp. 265-287). Berlin: Kluwer Academic Publishers. [Google Scholar]
Schacter, D. L. (2012). Constructive memory: Past and future. Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience, 14(1), 7-18. [Google Scholar] [PMC]
Schneeweis, N., Skirbekk, V., & Winter-Ebmer, R. (2014). Does education improve cognitive performance four decades after school completion? Demography, 51(2), 619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Shankar, A., Hamer, M., McMunn, A., & Steptoe, A. (2013). Social isolation and loneliness: Relationships with cognitive function during 4 years of follow-up in the English longitudinal study of ageing. Psychosomatic Medicine, 75(2), 161-170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Shigemori, K., Ohgi, S., Okuyama, E., Shimura, T., & Schneider, E. (2010). The factorial structure of the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) in Japanese dementia patients. BMC Geriatrics, 10(1), 36-42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Slavin, M. J., Brodaty, H., Kochan, N. A., Crawford, J. D., Trollor, J. N., Draper, B., & Sachdev, P. S. (2010). Prevalence and predictors of “subjective cognitive complaints” in the Sydney memory and ageing study. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(8), 701-710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Soininen, H. S., Partanen, K., Pitkänen, A., Vainio, P., Hänninen, T., Hallikainen, M., . . . Riekkinen, P. J. (1994). Volumetric MRI analysis of the amygdala and the hippocampus in subjects with age-associated memory impairment: Correlation to visual and verbal memory. Neurology, 44(9), 1660-1668. [Google Scholar]
Squire, L. R. (1992). Memory and the hippocampus: A synthesis from findings with rats, monkeys, and humans. Psychological Reviews, 99(3), 195-231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Strauss, E., Sherman, E., & Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of neuropsychological tests: Administration, norms, and commentary (3rd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
Tombaugh, T. N., & McIntyre, N. J. (1992). The Mini‐Mental State Examination: A comprehensive review. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 40(9), 922-935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Tucker-Drob, E. M., Johnson, K. E., & Jones, R. N. (2009). The cognitive reserve hypothesis: A longitudinal examination of age-associated declines in reasoning and processing speed. Developmental Psychology, 45(2), 431-446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Tulving, E. (1972). Episodic and semantic memory. In E. Tulving & W. Donaldson (Eds.), Organization of memory (pp. 381-403). New York: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
Tulving, E. (1995). Organization of memory: Quo vadis? In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), The cognitive neurosciences (pp. 839-847). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
Tulving, E., & Craik, F. I. M. (2000). The Oxford handbook of memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
Winocur, G. (1982). Learning and memory deficits in institutionalized and noninstitutionalized old people: An analysis of interference effects. In F. l. M. Craik & S. Trehub (Eds.), Aging and cognitive processes (pp. 155-181). New York: Plenum Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
World Medical Association. (2013). WMA Declaration of Helsinki - Ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. [Google Scholar] [URL]
Zahodne, L. B., Glymour, M. M., Sparks, C., Bontempo, D., Dixon, R. A., MacDonald, S. W. S., & Manly, J. J. (2011). Education does not slow cognitive decline with aging: 12-Year evidence from the Victoria longitudinal study. Journal of International Neuropsychological Society, 17(6), 1039-1046. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Zandi, T. (2004). Relationship between subjective memory complaints, objective memory performance, and depression among older adults. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 19(6), 353-360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Published
How to Cite
Issue
Section
License
Copyright (c) 2016 Helena Espirito-Santo, Inês Torres Pena, Inês Queiroz Garcia, Catarina Freitas Pires, Margarida Couto, & Fernanda Daniel
This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License.
The authors retain the copyright and grant the journal right of first publication with the work simultaneously licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License that allows the sharing of work and recognition of authorship and initial publication in this journal.